Page 1
Chapter
One
A Perspective on Adult Education
Research
Huey B.
Long
One's perception of a phenomenon
is determined in part by the angle and distance at which the phenomenon is
viewed. Because perspective has such an important effect on one's mental
images of phenomena, this chapter is designed to help readers establish a
common position from which they can view adult education research. Accordingly,
it has a fourfold mission: to look briefly at research incentives in adult
education, to define adult education research, to discuss its special
characteristics, and to review the relevant literature.
Research
Incentives
Several elements are interacting to generate incentives for
inquiry into adulthood and all of its social, psychological,
economic,
Page 2
and political significance. These
elements include our limited knowledge of adulthood; the peculiarities
of adulthood, which suggest that a subtle distinctiveness characterizes research
on this subject; and the rapid expansion of programs designed specifically
for adults.
Formerly, adults were often viewed in an undifferentiated manner.
Too little thought was given to how they differed from one another or from
children. And even with the. phenomenal increase in research concerning the
human life cycle, we still know little about the life stages; our knowledge
of the transitions from adolescence to middle age to senescence remains
embarrassingly limited. We are also insufficiently informed about how the
concepts of adult learning and continuing education have changed, as well
as about the changing cultural influences on adult behavior. We are even
short on information concerning how behavior is culturally conditioned. Because
of the significance of adulthood and the extent of our ignorance of
important life roles having to do with leisure, family life, and work,
substantial research is needed.
However, in seeking to answer questions about adulthood, we cannot
just shift labor and research instruments from a child population to an adult
population. The ways in which adults differ from children, the conditions
under which the study of adults is usually conducted, and the character of
the field in which the researcher is working (adult education as opposed
to psychology, for instance) all make research on adulthood subtly unique.
For one thing, researchers concerned with adult behavior-more than
researchers dealing with childhood-frequently have some kind of
relationship with the individuals from whom data are collected. In addition,
the questions investigated in adult education and related human resource
development fields are usually embedded in a complex content. And the
variables one wants to examine are often particularly difficult to isolate
and to observe in adult research. yet despite the challenges posed by these
characteristics, or perhaps because these qualities also provide incentive,
adult education researchers and others have been making substantial
progress.
Additional incentive is being offered by the elaboration and expansion
of programs intended to serve adult needs. The aspiration to provide exemplary
programs has focused attention on the limita-
Page
3
ions of the research results available to help program specialists
make sound decisions. Professional adult educators, trainers, and others
want to know more about the motives and goals of specific adults; about how
adults in certain age ranges handle interpersonal relations and conflicts;
about how they can more efficiently and effectively help an adult to develop
a new skill or understanding. Others are interested in improving delivery
systems and procedures, methods of finance, administrative procedures and
the like. The consequences of these developments is clearly a growing
demand for more and better information and therefore also for more and better
research, using methods specially suited to the study of
adults.
Defining
Adult Education Research
Definition is a device used to sort things into categories for purposes
of identification and communication. The more precise the definition, the
greater the restrictions on what can qualify for inclusion in any category
and the greater the possibility that different individuals will have the
same understanding about the identity of a phenomenon. Mental convergence,
then, is assisted by definition and contributes to improved communication.
Unfortunately, the term adult education research lacks a commonly
accepted precise definition. Each word in the term has been variously
defined, and perhaps no diverse absolute consensus is possible. Thus, rather
than attempt to explicate any of these words, I will merely provide a few
examples of definitions and share my own views.
Adult Education.
Taking first things first,
let us examine what is meant by adult education. Adult education has been
both narrowly and broadly defined according to a variety of criteria, from
the way it functions to the characteristics of its clients. The interested
reader is referred to Verner (1964, p. 30), Schroeder (1970, pp. 25-44) and
Spence (1955) for more detailed discussions.
Of the efforts to distinguish adult education from other kinds
of education,
those of Bryson (1936) and Verner are noteworthy. according to Bryson
(1936, pp. 3-4), adult education is all activities with an educational
purpose carried on by people in the ordinary business of life who use
only part of their time and energy to
Page 4
acquire intellectual equipment. Verner's
definition (1964, p. 32) is more restrictive: "Adult education is a relationship
between an educational agent and a learner in which the agent selects, arranges,
and continuously directs a sequence of progressive tasks that provide systematic
experience to achieve learning for those whose participation in such
activities is subsidiary and supplemental to a primary productive role in
society."
I prefer to remove the part-time and agent restrictions and define
adult education as any planned learning activity engaged in by and for anyone
who possesses the biological, civil, and cultural characteristics of
an adult. Admittedly, this definition is extremely broad, but so is the field
of adult education. Adding restrictive elements appears to be desirable only
when we want to specify certain kinds of adult education activities. Such
a procedure parallels the classification system used in biology: education
is the kingdom, adult is the phylum; the class, family, genus, and species
are to be determined through greater specification.
Research. The
definitional range of this word reflects the varying levels of sophistication
and complexity of the process and the research experience of the individual
using the word. For example, research may be broadly defined to include any
procedure used to collect information for the purpose of making a decision.
A more precise definition is the careful, disciplined, organized, and
exhaustive investigation of all ascertainable evidence bearing on a
definable problem (Hillway, 1974, p. 5). Other restrictions also are
frequently placed on the definition. Two such limitations concern the
generalizability of the findings and the purpose of the inquiry. That is,
research findings are expected to be generalizable to large populations
or to other similar phenomena, and research consists of inquiries that attempt
to assess the scientific truth of a thing as opposed to inquiries designed
to evaluate the worth of something (Glass, 1970).
Some writers suggest, however, that the crucial question is not whether
or why differences among definitions exist but what, if anything, is common
to all the activities defined as research. Thus the answer to the critical
question is to be found not in what is studied but in how the inquiry is
conducted (Aker and Schroeder, 1969). Research can then be distinguished
from other means that
Page 5
are
frequently used to answer a question or solve a problem, such as simply
letting change occur, adopting trial-and-error procedures, generalizing
from experience, or depending on mysticism, tradition,
and custom.
To answer the more important questions of reality, mankind has attempted
to develop more dependable ways than those noted above to determine truth.
Consequently, several methods for determining reality, resolving issues,
and answering questions have evolved, including appeals to authority,
philosophical and religious systems, and the scientific method. Each has
its adherents and detractors, of course, who emphasize either its strengths
or weaknesses. Though it is not the purpose of this chapter to examine and
discuss the relative merits of the different modes, a few comments drawn
from the literature that relate to the issue are included. Other chapters
in this book also touch on the reliability and status of different systems
of inquiry or problem resolution.
The Scientific Method.
This book is concerned
with the scientific method of determining what is real as opposed to the
other means mentioned above. The scientific method has several definite and
identifiable steps: (1) identifying the problem to be investigated,
(2) collecting the essential facts related to the problem, (3) developing
tentative explanations of the problem, (4) evaluating the explanations to
determine their relative congruence with the observations (facts), and (5)
selecting the most likely explanation (Hillway, 1974, p. 12). Important
assumptions on which scientific research is based are discussed in Chapter
Two.
Diverse problems have been solved by the use of the scientific
method. In pure research the investigator uses the method to discover new
knowledge about the universe. It is used in applied research to develop a
new product or process. And recently educators have used the scientific
method in what has been labeled action research to resolve practical problems
encountered in the learning environment.
Adult educators have found the scientific method helpful in improving
the concepts used in the field. For example, until about 1940 many adult
educators perceived adult education to be primarily for those people
with limited previous educational opportunity. The studies of Boshier (1971),
Johnstone and Rivera
Page 6
(1965), and others have demonstrated
overwhelmingly that the reverse is true, that the extent to which people
participate in adult education is positively correlated with the level of
their educational achievement. Londoner (1974) presents evidence that weakens
another previously held assumption, this one concerning the motivations
of students whose fees are paid by an agency. His findings cast doubt on
the assertions of some administrators that persons who have their fees paid
by an agency are unmotivated and generally unresponsive to academic programs
in the adult secondary schools and that their main reason for attending is
to receive financial aid. Of course, additional study is required to support or refute
his findings.
Thus, as the use of the scientific method provides new knowledge,
it concomitantly challenges old ideas and thereby makes humans more humble
when facing the unknown. No longer are they likely to believe they possess
a corpus of absolutely reliable knowledge that will provide complete,
authoritative answers to questions. Consequently, contemporary
investigators are generally openminded, for they are aware that revolutionary
advances made by science in the past century have challenged and overthrown
some long-accepted beliefs. Knowledge of those developments has encouraged
a flexible spirit of inquiry that makes it easier to question accepted theories.
Furthermore, such a spirit contributes to the investigators' humility, as
they recognize that their own discoveries too are
fallible.
Research concerning the ability of adults to learn provides a
quintessential illustration of the scientific method in action. Beginning
with the classic work of E. L. Thorndike .and others (1928), a series of
investigations spanning more than half a century has answered various questions
on this subject. And each major study has tended to generate more positive
findings and more generous interpretations concerning the adult's ability
to learn.
The scientific method of inquiry is not infallible, nor does it lead
to absolute certainty; but it is more reliable than some other means of answering
questions, which have been characterized (Cohen and Nagel, 1934, p. 195)
as inflexible, as containing no provision for error and correction. The
scientific method, in contrast, encourages doubt in order to make sure that
what is left after such critical assessment is supported by the best available
evidence. In
Page 7
addition to its fallibility, however,
the scientific method has other limitations. It is slow, it is not a perfect
method for discovering truth,
and it is limited in application
to certain kinds of questions. Conant (1947, p. 10) has declared that "only
an occasional brave man will be found nowadays to claim that the so-called
scientific method is applicable to the solutions of almost all the problems
of daily life in a modem world."
The rather lively debates that appear
in the learned journals, including Adult Education, reveal the controversy
concerning the scope of the applicability of the scientific method. Some
analysts suggest, for instance, that the scientific method is primarily suited
to the natural sciences and that its use should be limited accordingly. Others
question the concept of a single scientific method. There is rather general
agreement that one cannot establish a single rigid set of logical rules for
natural science, anthropology, mathematics, history, and education. Certainly
these fields differ; nevertheless, they have enough in common to reach a
unified scientific approach.
In spite of its limitations, the scientific method is one of the
most promising tools available to humankind to extend the frontiers
of knowledge and to increase the accumulation of tested and verified truth.
In the following discussion, therefore, adult education research is
conceptualized as the application of the scientific method to discovering
new knowledge about learners, content, curricula, activities, institutions,
and similar topics of concern to adult educators.
Special
Characteristics of Adult Education Research
Just as a range of differences distinguishes research in the natural
sciences from inquiry in the social sciences, so certain special characteristics
of research concerning adult education distinguish it from other educational
research. Investigators seeking to establish a data base for adult education
theory and practice soon become aware that their research problems are not
usually the same as those faced by other educational researchers. Some of
the questions may be similar, but both the populations and the educational
practices are often sufficiently different from those that most educators
and psychologists study to require modification of the research
activities.
Page 8
I have selected six characteristics
of adult-education research to illustrate this idea.
First, research that focuses on the adult period of life is complicated
by its relative length, with its attendant variables. Major research issues
may easily stretch across a population with a fifty-year range of
experience and cultural differences, in contrast to the much more restricted
age range studied by other educators. It should be obvious that the challenge
of designing appropriate investigations to comprehend such a span of time
adequately is not to be taken lightly. The confounding and complex research
issues are reflected in one of the oldest subjects of scientific inquiry
of interest to adult education, the problem of adult learning ability or
adult intelligence. Inquiries on this topic are replete with ambivalent,
equivocal, and apparently contradictory results and interpretations. Findings
are either supported and explained or discounted by various investigators
because they are longitudinal, cross-sectional, or cross-sequential.
Each design contains weaknesses that seem to be complicated by the combined
length and nature of this life period.
A second distinguishing characteristic is the lack of agreement on
what constitutes an adult. This lack of consensus interferes with communication
and the interpretation of data. For instance, the differing opinions concerning
the rates of participation in adult education activities can be traced directly
to this characteristic. Failure to agree on the definition of an adult has
contributed to large sample differences, and subsequently to substantial
differences among the estimates of participation. Three studies of adult
education participation reported in the literature use no fewer than
four different definitions of adults. Houle (1973, p. 66) cites an
unpublished work of Abraham Carp and Richard Peterson in which they
define their subjects as "eighteen through sixty living in their own homes
and not in full-time residence at a school or college." Johnstone and Rivera
(1965, pp. 31-32) used the following aggregates: "( 1) all householders
twenty-one years of age or over; or (2) under twenty-one but married; or
(3) under twenty-one but the head of a household; plus (4) all persons twenty-one
or over who live on an armed forces base and have close family ties with
some adult members of an American household; and (5) anyone twenty-one or
over and living in a school residence or dormitory and
closely
Page
9
related to some household member."
Finally, the survey data of the U.S. Office of Education (USOE) cover all
participants who are beyond
compulsory school age, seventeen
or over, who are not enrolled beyond full time in a regular school
or college program and are engaged in one or more activities of organized
instruction (National Center for Education Statistics,
1975).
Similarly, the third distinguishing characteristic, lack of agreement
on the properties of adult education, presents difficulty for the investigator
and scholar. The differences among the definitions contribute to difficulties
in communicating, comprehending, interpreting, and applying the findings
of various studies. For example, Verner's definition of adult education,
cited earlier, seems to exclude the self-directed learning activities studied
by Tough (1971); hence, using Verner's definition, we would eliminate the
independent learner from the concerns of adult education. If people do not
agree about what constitutes either an adult or adult education, there
is bound to be some disagreement also on what constitutes appropriate inquiry,
observation, and interpretation in the field.
A fourth distinguishing characteristics of adult education research
relates to questions of ethics and values. For example, investigators who
believe that the experimental research method manipulates people and that
such manipulation is always inappropriate will need to discover and
use different techniques, such as grounded theory. Investigators are thus
challenged to test their research objectives against philosophical and ethical
principles as well as the canons of logic.
The nature of the samples selected in the study of adult behavior is another element that makes such research distinctive. Investigators interested in adult behavior, like most researchers, have to be concerned with both internal and external validity. The character the adult population, however, presents particular difficulties. Researchers who seek a "normal" sample have some novel challenges, as do their colleagues who require a homogeneous sample. Because the normal adult population contains persons of widely different behavior, age, background, and so on, it is not easy to obtain what might be considered a normal randomly selected sample. Consequently, researchers often resort to one of two different strategies. They select subjects on the basis of their availability--that is,
Page 10
members of existing groups, clubs,
or organizations, and students--or they use extremely small samples.
And not infrequently they use both.
An analysis of a sample of eighteen studies using adult subjects
reported in volumes 24-27 of Adult Education revealed the following.
The sample size in the studies ranged from 11 to 728; seven investigations
were based on data from 200 or more subjects, and four of the studies had
a maximum of 20 subjects. Ten of the samples were composed of students. Nurses
made up the sample in three investigations, physical therapists in one, and
pharmacists in one. Former prison inmates and the alumni of a graduate program
in adult education were the subjects in two studies. Another investigation
reported a sample composed of members of six preexisting
groups.
If the frequent use of students, health care personnel, and other
available clusters of individuals, combined with small samples, is characteristic
of adult education research, this phenomenon may threaten the external validity
or generalizability of the research. However, new methods of statistical
analysis and interpretation may eventually strengthen our confidence in
small-sample studies. McKeachie and Kulik (1975) describe one recent innovative
procedure referred to as "vote counting" that may interest adult
educators. Vote counting is designed to further analyze studies comparing
the lecture and discussion methods of college teaching. Using this procedure,
McKeachie and Kulik (1975) went beyond an earlier review that considered
only the mean score on the final examination. They based their analysis on
three criteria: an examination, a measure of retention and higher level
thinking, and measures of attitudes and motivation. Disregarding statistical
significance levels, they compared the similarities and differences
of the two methods. Their results provide a picture different from the one
presented by the original analysis, which offered only the statistically
significant result. Another procedure that may prove useful to adult education
researchers is recommended by Gage (1978). This procedure, attributed to
Karl and Egon Pearson, tests the significance of combined results as a way
of overcoming some of the effects of small-sample
studies.
Diversity, a recognized characteristic of any group of
adults,
Page
11
presents a unique contrasting challenge
to the person trying to select appropriate
samples that will protect
the internal validity requirements of scientific investigations. For example,
investigators may find it extremely difficult to obtain a large sample matched
on selected characteristics or to obtain homogeneity desired within a specific
group of available adults. Faced with such a predicament, investigators have
only a few options, and according to strict research canons, some of these
are not desirable, In such cases researchers
may proceed and ignore the question
of homogeneity; they may continue the research on the assumption that the
members of the sample are similar; they may use a statistical procedure such
as analysis of covariance; or researchers may resort to using an extremely
small, but matched, sample and run the risk of a "type II error"--failing
to reject a null hypothesis that should be rejected.
The significance of this feature of adult-based research may not
be readily apparent, however, because of the heavy reliance adult educators
have placed on descriptive and correlational studies.
A sixth characteristic arises from the sophistication of an adult
sample. Data based on naive responses are difficult to obtain because the
educational and occupational experiences of a sizable segment of the available
adult population have made them increasingly sophisticated or test-wise.
Furthermore, investigators realize that adults are more likely than younger
respondents to attempt to provide the correct or valued response. For example,
I am familiar with a study wherein the adult respondents were requested to
describe, first on a written questionnaire and several weeks later in response
to in-depth interviews, the leisure activities of their neighbors. Not
surprisingly, the first set of responses followed certain middle-class
family-oriented values, whereas the second set of responses revealed
a much wider range of activities,
The
Literature
The beginnings of a literature on adult education research can be
traced to pre-World War II days; however, the literature of the past twenty-five
years is the most pertinent for the purposes of this chapter, Several threads
are discernible in the literature, Some writing relates research concerns
to graduate instruction. A number
Page 12
of bibliographic works identify the
research publications of interest to adult educators. And finally, the more
analytical and philosophical literature describes and criticizes the
status of adult education research and sometimes comments on what ought to
be.
Research and Graduate Study.
The limited literature on this aspect of the field suggests that
the development of competent researchers is an important goal of graduate
work. In the late fifties Whipple (1958) identified a pair of research needs
among adult educators: the need to search for truth as scientists and the
competing need to improve the practice of adult education. His solution
was to separate adult education from the graduate school and make it a
professional element in the university structure similar to schools of medicine
and law. During the early sixties, as graduate programs in adult education
began to proliferate, Liveright (1964) made some observations about the nature
and aims of the programs. Drawing on the work of William McGlothlin, he
identified five attributes common to all programs of graduate education,
one of which was that the graduate should possess enough competence to add
to human knowledge through either discovery or the application of new truths.
Liveright noted that all professions demand that their graduates will do
some kinds of continuing learning and require at least a minimal understanding
of relevant research. He then cited the expectations of the medical and social
work professions and concluded that adult education "can hardly settle for
less than the research requirements established by the medical profession"
(p. 99).
Douglah and Moss (1969, p. 132) succinctly stated the relationship
between research skills and graduate study:
The doctoral program should also be concerned with developing considerable
competence in research methodology. While this is an obvious need in the
case of those whose primary role will be research, it is also important
for those whose future roles may be primarily teaching. As professors of
adult education, their role will include advising students on research, and
thus they, as well as the researcher, will directly affect the quality of
adult education research performed. Since it is generally conceded that the
development of a field of study is
Page
13
linked to the quality and quantity of research done, the nature of
graduate training in this area should receive attention.
Boyd (1969) took a more controversial position concerning the content
of doctoral programs in adult education. He noted that since doctoral programs
are directly concerned with adult education as a subject of study, the emphasis
should be on research. Boyd noted, "our purpose is to observe, analyze, and
develop evidence and theory to explain that which we are examining and studying.
If by this process we develop excellent practicing professionals, all to
the good. . . . But it must be remembered that this is not our direct goal"
(p.190).
Research training in graduate programs is also recognized as contributing
to the professional competence of the adult educator. Aker (1962) and Houle
(1970) have commented on that relationship. Even though Houle's analysis
of adult education leadership does not specifically cite the performance
of competent research as among the four identified functions of leadership,
one can infer that such skill is required by individuals who can perform
his fourth function, namely, advancing adult education as a field of study.
Aker points out a need for greater skill among adult educators in identifying,
critically evaluating, and discussing scholarly work by investigators in
adult education.
Bibliographic Literature.
This category contains the greatest number of publications concerned
with adult education research including articles by DeCrow and Loague (1970),
Grabowski (1973), Grabowski and Loague (1970), Kaplan (1957), Thiede and
Draper (1963, 1964), and Thiede and Meggars (1965, 1966). The bibliographic
literature is useful to the field but has limited value in the discussion.
I mention it only to ensure that the reader is aware of
it.
Philosophical and Analytical Literature.
Among the contributions
to this second-largest body of adult education research literature are those
of Bittner (1950), Brunner and others (1959), Essert (1953), Hallenbeck (1964),
Hendrickson (1960), Jensen (1964), Kreitlow (1959, 1960, 1964a, 1964b, 1970),
Spence
Page 14
(1953), and Verner (1956). In addition, several complete issues of
the Review of Educational Research published between 1950 and 1965
focused on adult education. The 1950 and 1960 editions of the Encyclopedia
of Educational Research and the chapters by Kreitlow (1960, 1970) in
the Handbook of Adult Education provide a useful profile of the
development of adult education research. Those publications, supplemented
by Brunner's overview and the comments of Jensen, Liveright, and Hallenbeck
in their outlines of the field, provide an excellent base from which to examine
how research on and in the field of adult education has
grown.
Review
of Development
My review begins with an examination of the literature of the early
1950s, considers the predominance of descriptive research, identifies a period
of transition in which research activity began to proliferate, and
analyzes the dependence of adult education researchers on theory borrowed
from the traditional disciplines.
Early Work. The
earliest works, cited above, reflect a mixture of concern and optimism as
the fledgling field of adult education struggled to establish itself. Each
succeeding author generally expresses a little more optimism as research
products and skills emerge over the decades. Grabowski's chapter in the present
book exemplifies this attitude.
Early editions of the Review
of Educational
Research (Houle,
1953; Spence, 1950) were less optimistic about the status of adult education
research than were later descriptions and analyses. In the 1953 Review,
for example, Houle observed that research had long suffered from a lack
of information about activities in the field. In his opinion, even the simplest
questions frequently went unanswered. Accordingly, he cited the need for
basic bodies of facts that would provide a baseline for progress and a framework
for more detailed study. At the same time, however, Houle was heartened by
the improving quality of the literature and the increasing trend toward the
replacement of conjecture and unfounded assertion by scientific studies and
objective data.
Essert (1953), writing in the same issue of the
Review,
Page 15
described the period between 1949
and 1952 as a time when adult education was developing the
broad outlines of a substantive research program with an increasing
base of trained researchers and research
funding. He was not completely
optimistic, however, noting that with
respect to many important
questions in adult education there was little evidence of research and
experimentation. He observed, for example, that practically no significant
research had taken up the problems related to financing adult education.
Essert also cited the great need for research in adult education similar
to the many studies then being done on the growth and development of children
and adolescents.
In the same general period, Bittner (1950) noted the absence of a
clear and standard definition of adult education by any large group of
investigators or leaders in the "various associated movements" of adult
education. He referred to the dearth of basic research in the social sciences
by specialists in selected fields such as anthropology, sociology,
and social psychology as one reason for the divergence of opinion concerning
the definition of adult education. Bittner identified the two publication
series sponsored by the American Association for Adult Education, "Studies
in Adult Education" and "Studies in the Social Significance of Adult Education
in the United States," along with the Report of the Regent's Inquiry, the
publications of the Employment Stabilization Institute, and The
Literature of Adult Education, as examples of some of the better research
reports of the period. In conclusion, he predicted that research in adult
education would probably continue to be largely descriptive, derivative,
philosophical, and markedly applied in its orientation. Bittner gave two
reasons for his conclusions: first, the complexity and rapidity of change,
and second and more important, adult educators' failure to understand the
scientific method and cultural relativity (p. 32).
Approximately ten years later, the study of adult-education research
by Brunner and others (1959) noted that "any examination of research in adult
education reveals a rather chaotic situation. . . . A few pertinent
areas, such as adult learning, have been explored far more thoroughly than
others," while "some have received almost no research attention." Furthermore,
they observed that, other than in the field of methods, most research of
con-
Page 16
sequence had been conducted by social
scientists other than adult educators (p. 2).
Descriptive Research.
Kreitlow (1960) followed
closely, in time and content, the observations of Brunner and his colleagues.
The status of research in adult education was not high, he said, in terms
of either amount or quality. He closed the decade with an observation similar
to the one Bittner had made ten years earlier: "The last two decades of adult
education might be identified as the age of description" (p. 12). But he
was not pessimistic about the possible negative effects that the descriptive
studies might have on adult education research and, unlike Bittner, did not
associate the descriptive studies with any basic conceptual flaw common to
adult educators.
Kreitlow's sense of guarded optimism was evident in his writing four
years later (1964b), even as he stated that the giant steps needed in adult
education research remained to be taken. At that time, he pointed out that
even though important contributions had been made, the absence of structure
and lack of theory prevented the launching of new research and the integration
of previous work. To illustrate the benefits of developing rigorous structures
and models in adult education, he pointed to some eminently successful
studies of diffusion, innovation, and adoption that were constructed on such
foundations.
Later, Kreitlow (1970) indicated that although researchers were still
emphasizing "what" rather than "why," he was encouraged by the increasing
number and size of graduate programs. Their growth, he felt, indicated greater
attention among adult educators to research and theory. Still, he noted that
the research beginning to appear was "not in great quantity" nor did it have
"any consistent quality" (p. 138), descriptions reminiscent of his 1960
comments.
Transition. If
the two decades following World War II can be characterized as an era of
descriptive research in adult education, then the era beginning about 1966
can be characterized as the period when such research began to have a real,
though still relatively minor, impact on the literature and practice. Most
writers cited some forces that they believed had helped to improve adult
education research. These positive factors include support from certain
foundations (Carnegie, Ford, and Kellogg); the support and
en-
Page
17
couragement provided by the American Association for Adult Education and the Adult Education Association of the U.S.A; the development of the periodical Adult Leadership, its successor, Lifelong Learning: The Adult Years, and the Journal Adult Education; the expansion of graduate instruction programs in adult education; and the size and the increased significance of the Adult Education Research Conference, a loosely structured organization of adult educators interested in research who meet annually to present and discuss their research.
At the same time, other influences have impeded the development of
research. These factors include the pressures of practice, the youthfulness
of the field, the variety of institutional bases, the
traditional emphasis on descriptive
studies, and limited theory. The pressures of practice have frequently been
noted in the literature. Jensen (1964) likened them to the pressures experienced
in engineering, law, medicine, business and public administration, social
work, public health, and various other professions whose primary objective
is coping with some unsatisfactory condition or problem. In the earlier years
of development, adult educators were, perhaps out of necessity, more concerned
about what worked than about why something was effective.
This practical thrust, the emphasis
on doing instead of investigating or contemplating, not only affected
the character of the early literature but probably continues to influence
contemporary development as well. Hallenbeck (1964) described knowledge as
consisting of three elements-experience, research, and theory which came
into existence in that order. This sequence seems to hold true for adult
education knowledge.
The tendency to do, then report, generated a number of personal
narratives that lacked a high degree of generalizability. The next developmental
step was the publication of numerous status reports and other descriptive
studies. The movement from the experience-based personal reports to
descriptive studies was a natural and orderly development because of the
close relationship between
description and practice.
The limited generalizability and application of much of the early research
was due partly to the complications caused by the variety of institutions
in which adult education programs are located and to inadequate research
designs. This period
Page 18
of transition will end when a sufficient
number of graduates of adult education graduate programs have assumed
responsibility for the literature of their field.
Dependence on Other Disciplines.
Adult educators have generally looked to other disciplines for their
theoretical foundations. As Jensen (1964) and the Commission of the Professors
of Adult Education (1961) noted, adult education has been advanced through
the borrowing and adapting of knowledge, theory, and research technology
from other fields. While acknowledging this dependence, however, the Commission
cautioned that not all material from the social sciences is appropriate to
adult education and suggested that adult education should not only test the
applicability of existing knowledge but discover for itself new knowledge
or new relationships within existing knowledge. According to the Commission
members, adult education is original in developing special knowledge about
the unique characteristics of adults as learners. Yet, the development
of adequate knowledge about adults as learners and about the administration
of adult learning opportunities requires the involvement of far more
researchers than the relatively few who are now identified directly with
the field of adult education. They also stated that our knowledge of adult
education is limited because subjects of interest to adult educators
were often of secondary interest to other educators and social scientists
and because there was no mechanism for systematically planning, stimulating
and disseminating adult education research.
The dependence of adult education on other fields within education
and on the social sciences for research and theory came about for several
reasons. First, for a fledgling field, it made sense to use the knowledge
developed by one's parents and relatives, And second, of course, the early
professors had to come from somewhere, and in this case somewhere was usually
education and the social sciences. Third, because the group of scholars involved
was quite small-only twenty-four professors were listed on the Commission's
1961 roster, for instance-their resources were necessarily spread very
thinly.
Another probable cause was the great diversity of the field. Indeed,
Kreitlow asked in 1960 whether this diversity was such that research might
always have to be borrowed. Although Kreitlow
Page 19
probably
then considered a negative answer possible, believing that the borrowing
might continue only until adult education was more
defined in 1975 when he spoke
at a panel session at the Adult Education Research conference, he seemed
to have accepted the view that adult education would primarily build on a
foundation of research in the social science disciplines by interpreting
its implications for adult education. The suggestion made by the Commission
of Professors--that the unique characteristics of adults as learners could
and should be the focus of original research--appears to have been
overlooked by those who were conducting research on teaching, learning, and
administration of adult education or attitudes had changed in the
interim.
Mezirow (1971), however, took a different position concerning
the borrowing and adaptation. He indicated that adult education should have
its own theoretical structure based on research. He further speculated
that in the absence of theories suited to adult education as a professional
field, research efforts had been fragmented. Mezirow described most of the
research as (1) atheoretical or factual, (2) conceptual, (3) organizing
or critically evaluating existing factors, or (4) designed to test logical
deductions from assumptions, general formulations either from the literature
or from some element of formal theory couched in other
disciplines.
Thus, from about 1950 through 1970 much of the literature presented
adult education research as an adaptation of work in the social sciences.
Only a few voices were raised to argue for the development of theory
that would be unique to adult education.
Newer Approaches to Research.
Since the early seventies, when Kreitlow and Mezirow stated their
positions, others have offered differing opinions about the nature of adult
education research. Reflecting a concern similar to Kreitlow's, Beder
and Darkenwald (1974) picked up Whipple's theme (1958) and described
adult education as an applied professional field. As such, it needs two different
kinds of studies: basic research designed to extend knowledge and theory
and, equally important, research intended to help solve problems of policy
and practice.
Apps (1972) called for a broader definition of research, one that
moves beyond empirical inquiry. According to Apps, adult educators have been
caught up in believing that all knowledge
Page 20
comes from empirical research.
Accordingly, he encouraged the consideration of other paths to knowledge
such as thinking, synthesizing, sensing, and accepting--or, in Royce's terms
(1964), rationalism, intuitionism, empiricism, and authoritarianism,
respectively. Addressing himself to empiricism, Apps noted that a ritualistic
approach to empirical research could be particularly limiting. "Insistence
on rigor as defined by a right adherence to a ritualistic method may close
off many sources of insight and information" (p. 63).
Shillace (1973), following Apps and
The statements by Apps,
Reviews of the publications of adult educators do not generally
support the charge that adult education is overly empirical. Long and Agyekum
(1974), having analyzed Adult Education over a nine-year period,
1964-1973, concluded that approximately 60 percent of the articles were
descriptive statements of personal be1ief, and program descriptions.
Page 21
research. Furthermore, 48 percent
were reporting on descriptive surveys. Thus, these analyses indicate that
while empirical studies may have increased recently, such inquiry methods
do not yet dominate the practice of research. Apps and others may have gained
their impressions from hearing repeated assertions of the need for empirical
studies.
Although this literature review has focused on research methods,
at least one observation needs to be made concerning research topics. Kreitlow
(1975), having reviewed the topics studied between 1964 and 1975, noted that
unfortunately very little progress had been made in addressing the
questions he had identified as priorities in 1963-64. His explanations for
this limited progress suggest that adult educators responded to pressures
from university administrators and federal grants. Responsiveness to funding
agencies and organization leaders in identifying research problems
leads to a great deal of activity but does not seem to yield much in the
way of additions to the fundamental knowledge of the field. Action research
in adult education has not been of much value in the advancement of
theory.
To sum up, then: adult education research has been influenced by a number of factors over the past fifty years. Some of those factors have encouraged improved inquiry; others have retarded it. Positive forces such as an increase in the number of doctoral students, the stimulation provided by the increasing stability of the Adult Education Research Conference, the availability of a research publication outlet in the form of Adult Education, and a slowly accumulating body of knowledge that is expanding in depth and breadth are contending with the negative pressures of a practice-oriented discipline, the immaturity of the field, and some confusion about the source and existence of theoretical foundations in adult education In spite of, or perhaps because of, those opposing forces, adult education research has reached a level to justify the discussion of the various topics included in the following chapters.
January, 2005
Return to the top of this page.
Return to the first page.
Go to the Preface, Contents, Chapter Two, Chapter Three, Chapter Four, Chapter Five, Chapter Six, Chapter Seven, Chapter Eight, the Epilogue, References, or the Index