IDENTIFYING “SUCCESS” CHARACTERISTICS IN SELF-DIRECTED ADULT LEARNERS
Roger Hiemstra
Robert Judd
Adult and Extension Education
1978
[The research
reported in this paper was made possible by a mini-grant awarded by the
University of Mid-America (UMA), Central Office,
TOPICS COVERED IN THIS PAPER
INTRODUCTION |
Background |
Statement of the Problem |
Limitations of the Study |
Significance of the Study |
Objective of the Study |
Definition of Critical Concepts |
METHOD OF THE STUDY |
Phase 1 – Literature Review |
Phase 2 – Determination of Success Criteria |
Phase 3 – Analysis of Data |
Phase 4 – Development of a Research Framework |
REVIEW OF LITERATURE |
General Literature on the Adult as Learner |
Self-Directed Learning |
SUCCESS CRITERIA |
ANALYSIS OF DATA |
IMPLICATIONS |
NEEDED RESEARCH |
Defining Framework Terms |
Basic Research About Adult Students |
Basic Research Regarding Adult Educators/Researchers |
Basic Research on Adult Education Institutions |
BIBLIOGRAPHY |
APPENDIX A: IMPLICATIONS SUGGESTED IN THE LITERATURE |
APPENDIX B: REPORT FOR UMA STUDENTS |
INTRODUCTION
Background
Are we a self-directed learning society? The "discovery” in the past few years of the vast amount of learning by adults that takes place each year outside of the formal classroom would lead one to believe that we all are living in a self-directed learning society. Tough's (1971) seminal research on adult's learning projects increased our awareness of the enormity of self-directed learning. Subsequent research on learning projects by many individuals has provided indications that the high level of invo1vement by adults in self-directed learning activity is fairly consistent across populations and even societies irrespective of such variables as location, amount of education, age, economic status, and occupational history. Indeed, the entire non-traditional education movement and the evolvement of institutions like UMA are related at least in part to this desire by adults to be engaged in self-directed learning.
It may well be that adults always have been heavily engaged in numerous learning endeavors, much of which has been self-directed in nature. Indeed, before there were many institutionalized means for adult classes there were libraries, study groups, discussion clubs, and “Abraham Lincoln's” study by the light from the fireplace; American pioneers learning about traveling, survival, and agriculture in new lands through observation, experimentation, and experience; vast numbers of newly arrived United States residents learning English in self-study groups; and many other forms of self-directed adult learning. However, the research described above, the increasing pressure by adults for more learning opportunities and the awareness of diminishing national resources with related future implications for less travel to classroom settings are some of the pressures forcing more attention on self-directed learning. Subsequently, there is a real need for educators in higher education, adult education, and non-traditional education to respond in some real ways.
Statement of the Problem
As described above and as will be detailed in a later section, a great deal is known about the self-directed learner. Indeed, much of the development and success of the entire non-traditional education movement has been predicated on the notion that adults crave opportunities for learning that can exist outside the bureaucratic framework of traditional schools and institutions of higher education. However, very little is known regarding what characteristics of "success" are related to self-directed learning. Finding some answers to this question should enable educators to be more successful, themselves, as they strive to focus their planning and course development efforts in terms of effectiveness and efficiency.
Therefore, the specific problem under investigation in this study is to develop some understanding of what it means to be a "successful" adult learner and, more precisely, to identify some characteristics of success that are related to non-traditional or self-directed learning. Available data on self-directed learning will be reexamined in terms of some of the identified success characteristics.
Limitations of the Study
An obvious limitation is the difficulty in defining the term "success" and in finding means of measuring success. The literature reviewed provided only limited help as will be seen because the term is utilized in many different ways. However, as this study is developmental in nature it will provide a basis for future research and clarification of the term.
Another limiting factor is the fact that new data were not gathered to answer questions raised by the research problem. Time and resource constraints necessitated the analysis and reexamination of already available data. It is expected that the data utilized still will provide some insight regarding the relationships between successful adult learning and self-directed learning.
A final limitation stems from not knowing what direct relevance the study will have for such non-traditional education endeavors as course development, resource identification, and student facilitation. Therefore, the report contains an implication section, a suggested model for analyzing research needs, and a set of research questions. It is hoped that future resources will be made available to allow further study and to promote better understanding of both success and self-directed learning.
Significance of the
Study
The results of this study should be useful in a variety of ways. It has been an effort to better understand the evolving theory area of and concepts related to self-directed learning. Indeed, it is believed that the entire effort by institutions of higher education and other organizations to serve learners outside of the formal classroom setting will be greatly enhanced by research efforts of the nature of this study.
In the final analysis this and following studies should be useful in at least the following ways:
Objective of the
Study
The primary objective of the study is to obtain a more thorough understanding of the meaning of "success" as it relates to both self-directed learning and non-traditional education. Self-directed learning will be the primary focal point for study and from that study implications for non-traditional education will be drawn. The following questions served as the basis for the study:
Definition of Critical Concepts
The key concepts used in this study and some suggested definitions are outlined in the following sub-groups.
Adult learning. There may be as many unique definitions of adult learning as there are writers of the term. Each word in the two word concept elicits numerous definitional variations. Adult usually refers to a person who has reached some maturity level or responsibility for se1f and/or others. A related concept, adult education, usually refers to some relationship between this adult and some learning specialist or resource in an endeavor to learn something new. Learning, is generally accepted as the acquisition of knowledge, attitudes, and skills, usually resulting in some individual behavioral change. Thus, in this study adult learning refers to the process of information acquisition during adulthood made by individuals depending on needs, interests, learning skills, and resource availability.
Learning projects. The primary definitional basis for this term comes from the seminal work of Tough (1971). It refers to a series of clearly related learning efforts adding up to at least seven hours of effort within a six month period. Much of the current attention to self-directed learning stems from Tough's initial work.
Non-traditional education. Non-traditional education is a recognition that education should be measured by what the learner knows rather than by an educational process or institution. Therefore, non-traditional education is based on the premise that opportunity for learning should be equal for all who wish to learn, that learning is truly lifelong in nature., and that learning cannot be confined to one's youth or to formal classroom settings (Cross, 1975, p. 1).
Self-directed learning. "In its broadest meaning, self-directed learning describes a process in which individuals take the initiative, with or without the help of others, in diagnosing their learning needs, formulating goals, identifying human and material resources for learning, choosing and implementing appropriate learning strategies, and evaluating learning outcomes" (Knowles, 1975, p. 18). Such learning frequently is self-initiated and carried out alone.
Self-fulfillment learning activity. There are several subject matter groupings that have been utilized to classify the contents of learning projects. One such area, self-fulfillment, includes efforts at learning for leisure, arts and crafts, hobbies, and recreation. Thus, most learning efforts of a very personal nature can be considered, including study related to such areas as music, art, dance, theatre, religion, ethics, or moral behavior (Hiemstra, 1975, p. 5).
Success. To succeed according to the Random House dictionary, is to accomplish what is attempted or intended. In terms of self-directed learning and non-traditional education, success will imply satisfaction of accomplishment or in the participation act, itself, relative to the learning activity. The concept of success as discussed in the literature reviewed for this study will be described in considerable detail in a later section.
Type of learning planner. Key to understanding the concepts of self-directed learning and learning projects is the acceptance of the fact that the planning and facilitation of learning by adults can be quite varied. Several researchers who have examined the "learning projects" area have analyzed the type of planner used by adults for assistance in learning in the following categories: The learner himself or herself; a group or its leader/instructor; one person in a one-to-one situation; a non-human or material resource; and a "mixed" category where no dominant type of planner can be identified.
METHOD OF THE STUDY
The method followed in this study involved four different phases:
Phase 1- Literature Review
As would be expected, the literature directly or indirectly related to success in learning, self-directed learning, and non-traditional education is immense. A decision was made very quickly because of limited time and resources to limit the review to the following types of sources:
Obviously, much more literature still needs to be located and reviewed. One noticeable gap is literature of a psychological or sociological nature related to the "success" concept. In addition, numerous journals on such topics as educational gerontology, non-traditional education, and international adult education research contain relevant information waiting to be gleaned. Hopefully, subsequent research can include a review of some of these suggested literature sources.
Phase 2 - Determination of Success Criteria
The original intent and expectation was that obvious "success" criteria related to adult learning would be easily found. However, this was not the case in the literature reviewed. Subsequently, some synthesizing, conclusion drawing, and theoretical suggesting were necessary. Basically, success is looked at within the context of the individual learner and his or her learning activity.
Phase 3 - Analysis of Data
There are three sources of data utilized in this study: the
original learning projects data displayed or described by Tough (1971); the
study of older adults' learning projects in
The success criteria determined in Phase 2 will be examined where possible by a display of data from the original studies. In some cases the data has been reexamined and displayed in new tables. In some instances, the criteria are examined without available data but by suggesting how the known findings are related. In all cases, some explanation and discussion is included.
Phase 4 - Development of a Research Framework
One of the main products of the research effort was the realization that many more questions were being raised than were being answered. Correspondingly, the need arose to organize these questions in some useful way. Thus, a conceptual framework was conceived for categorizing the various questions. It is hoped that the framework and research suggestions will be a meaningful by-product in terms of future efforts by others to increase knowledge regarding all the areas studied.
REVIEW OF LITERATURE
General Literature on the Adult as Learner
Considerable research and writing about adult learning has been carried out during the past several years. This development has reached a level of sophistication such that there now exists a fairly sizable body of tested knowledge on adult learning and achievement. However, the purpose of this section will not be to outline that body of knowledge. Knox (1977a, 1977b) and Knox and Others (1968) do an excellent job of presenting such information. What this section will contain is a description of some of what is known about the adult as a participant in learning activities. Hopefully, such a picture will set the stage for understanding the self-directed adult and for examining such a learner in terms of several success criteria.
The adult as a participant in both formal and informal learning settings has been studied by several researchers. Boshier (1977) summarized considerable research on participation over several years with a factor analysis study. He suggests, for example, that young adults are more inclined to enroll for what he calls "External Expectations." Those who could be considered high in the amount of participation seem more inclined to be enrolled for escape or stimulation reason. He reports a variety of similar type of conclusions in the article.
The above researchers are cited only as examples of the wealth of literature available on the adult learner as a participant. Hiemstra (1976) summarizes some of these findings by suggesting that the adult participant tends to be younger, highly educated, highly motivated to learn, and highly skilled in social relationships (pp. 84-85). Other findings of this nature, a discussion of the adult drop-out, and a description of some barriers to participation are also included in the 1976 source.
Satisfaction and level of achievement also have been examined by several researchers. McLoughlin (1971) found that adult participation in course planning did not affect achievement but it did improve attitudes about the learning experience. Cole and Glass (1977) found that participation improved both achievement and attitude but it did not affect the retention of knowledge. Knox and Others (1968) determined that prior participation in adult education was associated with learning effectiveness. White and Hansen (1976) looked at means of increasing participant satisfaction and suggest several things the facilitator should consider. Again, the sources cited in this paragraph are only intended to provide the reader with a sense of the type of literature that is available.
One other area, because it is relatively new and gaining in attention by researchers, needs to be mentioned. Andrulis and Bush (1977) suggest that it is important adult learners be examined in terms of their cognitive styles. Meierhenry (1978), too, shows the need to examine cognitive styles in terms of their relationships to adult learning and other areas of research related to adults. Cross (1977) even suggests how certain cognitive styles may favor or be related to self-directed learning. This area of research has potential for understanding considerably more about the adult as learner and participant in adult or non-traditional higher education programs.
Self-Directed Learning
Considerable credit must be given to Baghi (1979) for the material contained in this section. The notion of self-directed learning is not a recent phenomenon. Tough (1967) and Kulich (1970) describe the emphasis on self-education throughout history. This concept emerged in adult education as self autonomous learning, self-directed learning, and self-planned learning in the 1960's. In theoretical terms, the work of Malcolm Knowles (1973, 1975) in self-directed learning is helpful.
Knowles refers to self-directed learning as "a process in which individuals take the initiative, with or without the help of others, in diagnosing their learning needs, formulating learning goals, identifying human and material resources for learning outcome" (1975, p. 18). He further suggests that other labels found in the literature to describe this process are "self-planned learning", "inquiry method", "independent learning", "self-direction", "self-instruction" and "autonomous learning." But the different labels are often mistakenly associated with the belief that learning is in isolation and the learner does all his/her activity on an entirely independent basis.
Tough (1971) in his explanation of self-planned learning points out that different labels such as self-education, self-instruction, self-teaching, independent study, self-directed learning, and individual 1earning "are somewhat similar to self-planned learning projects, but not identical" (1971, p. 42). He agrees that even though the learner may obtain help from a variety of human and material resources, the key to being a self-planned learner is carrying on the responsibility for the detailed decisions and arrangements associated with the learning activities. Hiemstra defines self-planned learning as "a learning activity that is self-directed, self-initiated and frequently carried out alone" (1976, p. 39).
Smith (1976) describes self-directed learning as having a special orientation to learning that "emphasizes the learner establishing and maintaining the major share of the responsibility for initiative and motivation in planning and carrying out his own learning activities" (1976, p. 3). The process includes diagnosing needs, formulating goals and choosing resources and methods. He further states that when the learner accepts this responsibility, the major consequences for him will be learning how to learn on his own or with a little assistance from others.
Knox (1973) suggests that a self-directed learner is the person who continues his learning "reflected in his selection of objectives that have high priority, followed by his selection from a range of learning activities that are most appropriate for the specific circumstances he confronts." For self-directed learning he suggests the following resources: Printed media, electronic media, informal groups, formal groups, and tutorial schedules.
The nature of self-planned learning is consistent with a basic characteristic of adults as self-directing human beings. Nevertheless, as Knowles (1975) points out, adults are not adequately prepared for this type of learning. Then he cites Kidd and suggests that the purpose of education should be producing "a continuing, inner-directed, self-operating learner" (Kidd, 1975, p. 47). The phenomenon of self-directed learning which has been explained by Tough (1967, 1971) and supported by many researchers who replicated his efforts started with an examination of the "learning project" or "a major learning effort." He defines the learning project as “a series of related episodes, adding up to at least seven hours within a consecutive 6-month period. In each episode, more than half of the person's total motivation is to gain and retain certain fairly clear knowledge and skills, or to produce some other lasting change in himself. For convenience the short hand label 'learning project' has been adapted to refer to this series of related episodes: a sustained, highly deliberate effort to learn" (1971, p. 6).
The focus of this learning project phenomenon includes the following basic components:
Tough and his associates (1971) conducted a survey of learning projects 66 persons selected from seven populations: social science professors, municipal politicians, lower-class white-collar men, blue collar factory workers, lower-class white-collar women, beginning elementary school teachers, upper-middle class women with preschool children.
The findings are summarized as follows:
The above research has also revealed that a large proportion of adults are engaged in highly deliberate learning efforts outside of educational institutions. Such a finding has a direct relationship to the non-traditional education movement. The many subsequent research efforts are detailed very highly in the following paragraphs.
McCatty (1973) studied learning projects of 54 randomly
selected professional persons (engineering, law, education, medicine,
architecture and science) in
Johns (1973) studied the learning projects of 39 pharmacists
from
In another study Fair (1973) examined the learning projects conducted by 35 first year elementary school teachers who were selected randomly from two school districts in Ontario, Canada. Fair discovered that the teachers conducted an average of 8.8 learning projects during the 26 weeks preceding the interview. Each project lasted for approximately 57 hours. Ninety-seven percent of these learning projects were self-planned. Less than one percent of the projects were for credit applied toward a degree or certificate.
Allerton (1974) studied the learning projects of 12 parish
ministers in the
Miller and Botsman (1975) studied the continuing education activity of Cooperative Extension agents. It was found that the average number of learning projects per agent was 12. Forty percent of these learning projects were self-planned. More than half of their learning was planned by experts and through workshops.
Kelly (1976) studied the learning projects of two groups of
secondary teachers from
McCatty (1976) investigated the patterns of learning projects among physical and health education teachers. He found that the learning efforts of those teachers were largely self-planned and not for credit.
Miller (1977) identified the nature and extent of
self-directed learning undertaken by teachers and non-teaching professionals in
a single school district in upstate
Benson (1974) studied the learning projects of fifty
randomly selected college and university administrators in
Zangari (1978) studied the learning projects conducted over
a one year period by 45 adult educators in post-secondary institutions in
Armstrong (1971) found a significant number of learning
projects among adults of low educational attainment in
Johnson (1973) investigated the learning projects of 40
adults who had just completed their senior high school examinations in
Coolican (1973) studied the learning project of 48
Hiemstra (1975) studied the learning project activity of 214
adults (age 55 and older) in
Peters and Gordon (1974) studied the learning projects of
466 adults in urban and rural
Umoren (1978) explored the learning activities of 50 adults
randomly selected from a socio-economic group in two neighborhoods in
Denys (1973) studied the learning efforts of 40 randomly
selected professionals (20 were secondary school teachers, 20 were store
managers) in
Field (1977) studied the learning efforts of 85 adults of
low literacy attainment in the Brownstown area in
The combination of the findings of all these studies shows that the differences among several populations are not great. The findings in each study are roughly similar with findings in other studies. The large differences are not among populations; they are within the given populations. The findings which have been summarized by Tough (1977) are as follows:
Looking at the above composite findings, Tough (1978) argues that until recently researchers looked only at the tip of the iceberg. In adult education the visible portion of the iceberg is primarily learning in classrooms, workshops, auditoriums, or conferences, tutorial or correspondence study, and programmed instruction. But what has been unnoticed until fairly recently, the invisible portion of the iceberg, is self-planned learning. Looking at adult education efforts in terms of the whole body of the iceberg, the conclusion can be made that adult education institutions could not possibly meet all the learning needs of adults through their traditional programming services. Therefore, adult and non-traditional education professionals must develop efficient and effective approaches for assisting adults with their deliberate self-planned learning efforts outside the traditional realm.
SUCCESS CRITERIA
Is there even such a phenomenon as self-directed learning? The previous section reviewed all available literature on learning projects research. Certainly, the research reveals a high level of learning activity by adults. Gooler (1975) suggests that to determine success one must decide what criteria to use, gather related evidence through some measure, and compare the evidence against the criterion standard. Although the above literature citations do not specifically spell out success criteria in terms of learning projects, the excitement generated by the research implies that some important information is emerging. Thus, in this ex post facto comparison of available data a success criterion is offered as an initial means for evaluating the learning projects research data:
· To what extent are adults engaged in learning?
Directly related to the above statement and of primary importance to this study is the nature of the involvement by individuals in learning. For example, Zahn (1967) provides some evidence to suggest that highly rated, self-sufficient adults tend to be well educated, from upper middle class families, cosmopolitan in nature, and more job-oriented then family oriented. Hiemstra (1976) reviewed the literature on adult education participation and summarized the findings on active participants by noting they tended to be younger, highly educated, middle class, and urban in terms of their place of residence. Thus, another success criterion is offered as follows:
· To what extent do the participants in Learning Projects differ from each other in terms of the amount of 1earning undertaken?
Another obvious success screen through which the learning projects data must be examined is the nature of the planning activity prior to and during learning. For example, do adults prefer learning in groups or by themselves? Tough (1971, p. 93) suggests that the self-reliant, independent type of person is likely to prefer self-planning as the primary learning mode. Therefore, the following criterion is offered:
· To what extent do adults select themselves as the primary planner in carrying out learning projects?
Means for examining the actual success of individual, self-directed adult learners can be extracted from the suggestions of several authors. For example, Knowles (1975) suggests that self-directed learners are motivated by internal incentives such as a need for self-esteem, a desire to achieve, and the satisfaction that will come from accomplishing something. Tough (1971) suggests something similar in his list of reasons as to why self-planned learning is popular and why it is selected by certain individuals:
Guglielmino (1977) has developed a self-directed learning readiness scale. Through a factor analysis procedure she isolated eight factors in self-directed learning:
A success criterion offered to examine this concept is as follows:
· To what extent do self-directed learners carry out learning projects of a self-fulfillment nature?
Another means for examining success can be extracted from discussion about resources utilized for the learning endeavor. Zangari (1978), for example, found that reading material was the most utilized resource for learning. Tough suggests that one of the reasons certain learners select self-planned learning as their approach is because of their ability to locate and utilize printed materials. Meierhenry (1977), too, urges that more be understood about the ability of field independent learners to place structure and organization on learning resources. Therefore, another criterion is offered:
· To what extent do self-directed learners utilize printed and other non-human resources in their learning endeavors?
This study represents only a first step in conceptualizing the notion of "success" in adult learning and in drawing some comparisons between success and self-directed learning. Much work and research remains to be completed if the research direction is to aid program planners, adult education teachers, and non-traditional education administrators. There are many more success criteria to be developed and tested. However, it is expected that this study will provide a basis for further research and examination.
ANALYSIS OF DATA
The first criterion asks the question, to what extent are adults engaged in learning. The seminal research by Tough (1971) and subsequent work by many have revealed that a heavy involvement in learning by most adults is taking place. Specifically for this section, the work by Tough, Hiemstra (1975), and Zangari (1978) will be examined.
Table 1 shows the comparison between these three studies on several basic areas of study. In addition, a composite picture offered by Tough (1977) is provided. Generally, the data reveal that adults are heavily engaged in learning, perhaps spending an average of 500 hours a year in learning. Note, too, the heavy reliance on self as a planner in the learning activity. (The self as planner will be examined in greater detail later in this section.) Therefore, in terms of this success criterion, the heavy involvement of most adults in learning can be verified.
Table 1. A Comparison of Summary Data from the Hiemstra, Tough, and Zangari Studies.
Data Description |
Hiemstraa |
Toughb |
Zangaric |
Composited |
Number of Learning Projects: |
|
|
|
|
Mean |
03.3 |
08.3 |
07.2 |
05.0 |
Median |
03.0 |
08.0 |
07.0 |
n.a. |
Range |
1-9 |
0-20 |
2-21 |
0-35 |
Percent of Participation in Learning Project Activity: |
84% |
98% |
100% |
90% |
Number of Hours of Participation: |
|
|
|
|
Mean |
325 |
816 |
583 |
500 |
Median |
237 |
687 |
445 |
n.a. |
Range |
12-2300 |
0-251 |
129-2830 |
0-6200 |
Current Status of Projects: |
|
|
|
|
Active |
75% |
66% |
75% |
70% |
Inactive/Completed |
25% |
34% |
25% |
30% |
Credit Status of Projects: |
|
|
|
|
Credit |
04% |
01% |
03% |
05% |
Non-Credit |
96% |
99% |
97% |
95% |
Planner Type: |
|
|
|
|
Self-planned |
55% |
68% |
72% |
73% |
Group planned |
20% |
12% |
15% |
14% |
One-to-one |
10% |
08% |
08% |
10% |
Resource Planned |
04% |
03% |
02% |
03% |
Mixed |
10% |
09% |
03% |
n.a. |
aHiemstra (1975), older adults, N = 214.
bTough (1971), seven different adult populations, N = 66.
cZangari (1978), adult educators, N = 45.
dComposite, Tough (1977)’s approximated figures from all studies or approximations by examining all the data.
However, are there differences among adults in the amount of learning undertaken (criterion two)? Table 2 compares those engaged in one or more learning .projects each year. Generally, it can be suggested that the more educated, higher social class, younger, and urban located individuals appear to be the most heavily engaged in learning. Although only the Hiemstra (1975) data are displayed the Tough (1971) and Zangari (1978) findings tend to support those shown in the table. In addition, the literature cited earlier agrees with the findings. Thus, from the standpoint of the success criterion, differences can be found and in the predictable directions.
Table 2. Crossbreak Comparisons of Selected Study Demographic Variables with the Level of Learning Project Activity.a
Comparison Variables |
No Learning Projects Number |
No Learning Projects Percent |
One or More Projects Number |
One or More Projects Percent |
Gender: |
|
|
|
|
Female |
026 |
061.9 |
125 |
058.4 |
Male |
016 |
038.1 |
038 |
041.6 |
χ2 = 0.06; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Race: |
|
|
|
|
White American |
042 |
100.0 |
185 |
086.4 |
Other |
000 |
000.0 |
029 |
013.6 |
χ2 = 5.14; Sig. = < .05 |
|
|
|
|
Marital Status: |
|
|
|
|
Married |
022 |
052.4 |
140 |
065.4 |
Widowed |
014 |
033.3 |
051 |
023.8 |
Single/Divorced |
006 |
014.3 |
023 |
010.7 |
χ2 = 2.58; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Years of Education: |
|
|
|
|
Less than 8th Grade |
002 |
004.8 |
022 |
010.3 |
8th – 11th Grade |
017 |
040.5 |
045 |
021.1 |
High School Graduate |
017 |
040.5 |
065 |
030.5 |
Some College |
004 |
009.5 |
033 |
015.5 |
College Graduate |
001 |
002.4 |
024 |
011.3 |
Graduate Training |
001 |
002.4 |
024 |
011.3 |
χ2 = 14.15; Sig. = < .05 |
|
|
|
|
Community: |
|
|
|
|
Urban |
019 |
045.2 |
126 |
058.9 |
Rural |
002 |
004.8 |
036 |
016.8 |
|
021 |
050.0 |
052 |
024.3 |
χ2 = 12.73; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
Age: |
|
|
|
|
55-64 |
010 |
023.8 |
091 |
042.5 |
65 and older |
032 |
076.2 |
123 |
057.5 |
χ2 = 5.14; Sig. = < .05 |
|
|
|
|
Social Class: |
|
|
|
|
Lower |
001 |
002.4 |
014 |
006.5 |
Lower Middle |
031 |
073.8 |
085 |
039.7 |
Upper Middle |
008 |
019.0 |
101 |
047.2 |
Upper |
002 |
004.8 |
014 |
006.5 |
χ2 = 16.75; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
Living Arrangement: |
|
|
|
|
Apartment |
004 |
009.5 |
028 |
013.1 |
House |
034 |
081.0 |
159 |
074.3 |
Other |
004 |
009.5 |
027 |
012.6 |
χ2 = 5.14; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
aSource: Hiemstra (1975). The source contains an explanation of terms and category combinations.
The third criterion calls for an examination of the learning project data in terms of the type of planner selected. As tables 3a, 3b, and 4 show, the learner himself or herself is the overwhelming choice as the primary planner. As a success criterion, this intense involvement of self as the primary planner indicates that the notion of self-directed learning is a real phenomenon. In addition, it can be noted that little differences in the amount of self-directedness is observable between various demographic sub-categories.
Table 3a. Types of Planners Involved in All Learning Projects.a
Planner |
Number |
Percent |
The Learner Himself/Herself |
234 |
72 |
A Group or its Leader/Instructor |
048 |
15 |
One Person in a One-To-One Situation |
027 |
08 |
A Non-Human Resource |
006 |
02 |
Mixed (No Dominant Type of Planner) |
009 |
03 |
aSource: Zangari (1978).
Table 3b. Frequency of Type of Primary Planners of Learning Projects.a
Primary Planner of Project |
Number With At Least One Project |
Average Number With Planner |
Tough Data |
A Group or its Leader/Instructor |
086 |
1.69 |
32 |
One Person in One-To-One Situation |
048 |
1.52 |
31 |
Material/Non-Human Resource |
022 |
1.27 |
08 |
The Learner Himself or Herself |
183 |
2.14 |
63 |
Mixed (No Dominant Type) |
046 |
1.57 |
26 |
aSources: Hiemstra (1975) and Tough (1971).
Table 4. Crossbreak Comparisons of Selected Demographic Variables with the Amount of Self-Directedness.a
Comparison Variables |
High Self-Directedness No.b |
High Self-Directedness % |
Low Self-Directedness No. |
Low Self-Directedness % |
Gender: |
|
|
|
|
Female |
060 |
48.0 |
065 |
52.0 |
Male |
052 |
58.4 |
037 |
41.6 |
χ2 = 1.87; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Race: |
|
|
|
|
White American |
102 |
55.1 |
083 |
44.9 |
Other |
010 |
35.7 |
019 |
64.3 |
χ2 = 4.78; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Marital Status: |
|
|
|
|
Married |
077 |
55.0 |
063 |
45.0 |
Widowed |
026 |
51.0 |
025 |
49.0 |
Single |
008 |
50.0 |
008 |
50.0 |
Divorced/Separated |
001 |
14.3 |
006 |
85.7 |
χ2 = 4.53; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Years of Education: |
|
|
|
|
Less than 8th Grade |
005 |
22.7 |
017 |
77.3 |
8th – 11th Grade |
029 |
64.4 |
016 |
35.6 |
High School Graduate |
038 |
58.5 |
027 |
41.5 |
Some College |
019 |
57.6 |
014 |
42.4 |
College Graduate |
013 |
54.2 |
011 |
45.8 |
Graduate Training |
007 |
29.2 |
017 |
70.8 |
χ2 = 16.90; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
Community: |
|
|
|
|
Urban |
064 |
50.8 |
062 |
49.2 |
Rural |
027 |
75.0 |
009 |
25.0 |
|
021 |
40.4 |
031 |
59.6 |
χ2 = 10.51; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
Age: |
|
|
|
|
55-64 |
043 |
38.4 |
048 |
47.1 |
65 and older |
069 |
61.6 |
054 |
52.9 |
χ2 = 1.93; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Social Class: |
|
|
|
|
Lower |
007 |
50.0 |
007 |
50.0 |
Lower Middle |
045 |
52.9 |
040 |
47.1 |
Upper Middle |
053 |
52.5 |
048 |
47.5 |
Upper |
007 |
50.0 |
007 |
50.0 |
χ2 = .07; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Living Arrangement: |
|
|
|
|
Apartment |
011 |
39.3 |
017 |
60.7 |
House |
090 |
56.6 |
069 |
43.4 |
Institution |
008 |
44.4 |
010 |
55.6 |
Other |
003 |
33.3 |
006 |
66.7 |
χ2 = 4.8; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
aSource: Hiemstra (1975).
bThose individuals with more selections of “self” as primary planner than the overall mean number of “self” selections.
Future research and program planning must deal with this phenomenon. Most importantly, the fact that success may be tied to self-initiated and self-planned learning and much less tied to say the teacher as instructor or to the use of something like a televised lesson (non-human resource) indicates that adult and non-traditional educators may need to go "back to the drawing board."
The fourth criterion calls for an examination of the learning projects in terms of the nature of their content. Obviously, in an ex post facto examination some conceptual liberties are taken because such measures as self-concept or satisfaction were not precisely measured. However, as Table 5 shows, there was a heavy overall preference for "self-fulfillment." Table 6 also reveals that there are some differences in the -amount of "self-fulfillment" involvement among different demographic groupings.
Table 5. Comparison of Subject Matter Area By Various Demographic Variables.a
Comparison Variables |
Occupational/ Vocational No. |
Occupational/ Vocational % |
Personal/ Family No. |
Personal/ Family % |
Social/ Civic No. |
Social/ Civic % |
Self-Fulfillment No. |
Self-Fulfillment % |
Age: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
55-64 |
085 |
27.33 |
072 |
23.15 |
023 |
07.40 |
131 |
42.12 |
65 and older |
030 |
07.50 |
072 |
18.00 |
044 |
11.00 |
254 |
63.50 |
χ2 = 62.01; Sig. = < .001b |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Community: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
080 |
18.48 |
095 |
21.94 |
047 |
10.85 |
211 |
48.73 |
Rural/Non-Town |
022 |
16.42 |
025 |
18.66 |
008 |
05.97 |
079 |
58.96 |
Rural/Small Town |
013 |
09.03 |
024 |
16.67 |
012 |
08.33 |
095 |
65.97 |
χ2 = 13.60; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Gender: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Male |
076 |
26.86 |
050 |
17.67 |
020 |
07.07 |
137 |
48.41 |
Female |
039 |
09.11 |
094 |
21.96 |
047 |
10.98 |
248 |
57.94 |
χ2 = 40.34; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Race: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
White American |
110 |
18.12 |
109 |
17.96 |
064 |
10.54 |
324 |
53.38 |
Black American |
000 |
00.00 |
003 |
100.00 |
000 |
00.00 |
000 |
00.00 |
Mexican American |
005 |
04.95 |
032 |
31.68 |
003 |
02.97 |
061 |
60.40 |
χ2 = 26.52; Sig. = < .001 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Social Class: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Lower |
004 |
10.53 |
008 |
21.06 |
000 |
00.00 |
026 |
68.42 |
Lower Middle |
032 |
12.90 |
061 |
24.60 |
021 |
08.47 |
134 |
54.03 |
Upper Middle |
062 |
17.82 |
061 |
17.53 |
030 |
08.62 |
195 |
56.03 |
Upper |
017 |
22.08 |
014 |
18.18 |
016 |
20.78 |
030 |
38.96 |
χ2 = 9.93; Sig. = < .05 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Living Arrangement: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Apartment |
023 |
12.07 |
022 |
19.82 |
017 |
15.32 |
049 |
44.14 |
Home |
087 |
11.76 |
104 |
21.05 |
036 |
07.29 |
267 |
54.05 |
Institution |
000 |
00.00 |
008 |
11.10 |
014 |
19.18 |
051 |
69.86 |
Other |
005 |
15.15 |
010 |
30.30 |
000 |
00.00 |
018 |
54.55 |
χ2 = 14.70; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Marriage Status: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Married |
085 |
18.44 |
087 |
18.87 |
026 |
05.64 |
263 |
57.05 |
Widowed |
015 |
08.98 |
043 |
25.75 |
025 |
14.97 |
084 |
50.30 |
Single |
007 |
10.94 |
008 |
12.50 |
013 |
20.31 |
036 |
56.25 |
Divorced/Separated |
008 |
42.11 |
006 |
31.58 |
003 |
15.79 |
002 |
10.53 |
χ2 = 11.64; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Education: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Less than 8th Grade |
002 |
02.90 |
021 |
30.43 |
001 |
01.45 |
045 |
65.22 |
8-11th Grade |
011 |
10.00 |
020 |
18.18 |
013 |
11.82 |
066 |
60.00 |
High School Grad. |
026 |
12.68 |
043 |
20.98 |
022 |
10.73 |
114 |
55.61 |
Some College |
024 |
17.65 |
023 |
16.91 |
011 |
08.09 |
078 |
56.35 |
College Graduate |
016 |
17.98 |
019 |
21.35 |
012 |
13.48 |
042 |
47.19 |
Graduate Training |
036 |
35.64 |
018 |
17.82 |
008 |
07.92 |
039 |
38.61 |
χ2 = 26.59; Sig. = < .001 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Occupation: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Highest Professional |
013 |
27.08 |
006 |
12.50 |
006 |
12.50 |
023 |
47.92 |
Lower Professional |
040 |
26.14 |
029 |
18.95 |
012 |
07.84 |
072 |
47.06 |
Administrative Pers. |
024 |
37.50 |
012 |
18.75 |
004 |
06.25 |
024 |
37.50 |
Homemaker |
009 |
04.23 |
051 |
23.94 |
022 |
10.33 |
131 |
61.50 |
Clerical/Sales/Tech. |
010 |
10.53 |
024 |
25.26 |
003 |
03.16 |
058 |
61.05 |
Skilled Manual |
009 |
27.27 |
014 |
16.28 |
011 |
12.79 |
052 |
60.47 |
Semi-Skilled/Operative |
009 |
27.27 |
044 |
12.12 |
003 |
09.09 |
017 |
51.52 |
Unskilled |
001 |
07.69 |
004 |
30.77 |
000 |
00.00 |
008 |
61.54 |
χ2 = 34.33; Sig. = < .001 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
aSource: Hiemstra (1975).
bChi-square values are based on the collapsed categories as displayed in Table 9. Percentages are based on comparison variable sub-category totals.
Table 6. Crossbreak Comparisons of Selected Demographic Variables with the Level of Self-Fulfillment Learning Project Activity.a
Comparison Variables |
One or Fewer Self-Fulfilment Learning Projects No. |
One or Fewer Self-Fulfilment Learning Projects % |
Two or More Self-Fulfilment Learning Projects No. |
Two or More Self-Fulfilment Learning Projects % |
Gender: |
|
|
|
|
Female |
080 |
55.9 |
071 |
62.8 |
Male |
063 |
44.1 |
042 |
37.2 |
χ2 = .97; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Race: |
|
|
|
|
White American |
135 |
94.4 |
092 |
31.4 |
Other |
008 |
05.6 |
021 |
18.6 |
χ2 = 12.81; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
Marital Status: |
|
|
|
|
Married |
089 |
62.2 |
073 |
64.6 |
Widowed |
033 |
23.1 |
032 |
28.3 |
Single |
014 |
09.8 |
007 |
06.2 |
Divorced/Separated |
007 |
04.9 |
001 |
00.9 |
χ2 = 4.98; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Years of Education: |
|
|
|
|
Less than 8th Grade |
009 |
06.3 |
015 |
13.3 |
8th – 11th Grade |
043 |
30.3 |
019 |
16.8 |
High School Graduate |
047 |
33.1 |
035 |
31.0 |
Some College |
016 |
11.3 |
021 |
18.6 |
College Graduate |
013 |
09.2 |
012 |
10.6 |
Graduate Training |
014 |
09.9 |
011 |
09.7 |
χ2 = 10.46; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Community: |
|
|
|
|
Urban |
084 |
58.7 |
061 |
54.0 |
Rural |
014 |
09.8 |
024 |
21.2 |
|
045 |
31.5 |
028 |
24.8 |
χ2 = 6.82; Sig. = < .05 |
|
|
|
|
Age: |
|
|
|
|
55-64 |
062 |
43.4 |
037 |
32.7 |
65 and older |
081 |
56.6 |
076 |
67.3 |
χ2 = 2.56; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Social Class: |
|
|
|
|
Lower |
007 |
04.9 |
008 |
07.1 |
Lower Middle |
074 |
51.7 |
042 |
37.2 |
Upper Middle |
052 |
36.4 |
057 |
50.4 |
Upper |
010 |
07.0 |
006 |
05.3 |
χ2 = 6.70; Sig. = N.S. |
|
|
|
|
Living Arrangement: |
|
|
|
|
Apartment |
019 |
13.3 |
013 |
11.5 |
House |
115 |
80.4 |
078 |
69.0 |
Other |
009 |
06.3 |
022 |
19.5 |
χ2 = 11.95; Sig. = < .01 |
|
|
|
|
aSource: Hiemstra (1975).
Zangari (1978) compared those projects where "self" was selected as the primary planner with those projects where other than "self" was selected on several affective types of measures. He found that self-directed learners generally feel they have learned a great deal (Table 7), were very enthusiastic about their learning (Table 8), and expressed a high degree of benefit from the learning endeavor (Table 9). It should be noted that in each of these three comparisons, the "mixed" category received the highest response rating indicating that some mixture of self-directed learning with other modes may be the best arrangement in promoting success.
Table 7. Degree of
Knowledge or Change for All Learning Projects Identified by Type of Planner.a
Response |
Self No. |
Self % |
Group No. |
Group % |
One-to-One No. |
One-to-One % |
Non-Human No. |
Non-Human % |
Mixed No. |
Mixed % |
Learned a large amount or
changed a great deal |
140 |
60 |
024 |
50 |
012 |
44 |
005 |
83 |
008 |
89 |
Learned a medium amount or
changed moderately |
084 |
36 |
015 |
31 |
010 |
37 |
001 |
17 |
001 |
11 |
Learned a small amount or
changed very little |
010 |
04 |
009 |
19 |
005 |
19 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
aSource: Zangari (1978).
Table 8. Degree of
Enthusiasm Expressed for All Learning Projects Identified by Type of Planner.a
Response |
Self No. |
Self % |
Group No. |
Group % |
One-to-One No. |
One-to-One % |
Non-Human No. |
Non-Human % |
Mixed No. |
Mixed % |
Very enthusiastic |
173 |
74 |
027 |
56 |
015 |
56 |
004 |
67 |
008 |
89 |
Fairly enthusiastic |
055 |
23 |
018 |
38 |
010 |
37 |
002 |
33 |
001 |
11 |
Not especially enthusiastic |
006 |
03 |
003 |
06 |
002 |
07 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
aSource: Zangari (1978).
Table 8. Degree of
Benefit for Others Expressed for All Learning Projects Identified by Type of
Planner.a
Response |
Self No. |
Self % |
Group No. |
Group % |
One-to-One No. |
One-to-One % |
Non-Human No. |
Non-Human % |
Mixed No. |
Mixed % |
Fairly large extent |
135 |
58 |
025 |
52 |
004 |
15 |
005 |
83 |
008 |
89 |
Medium extent |
066 |
28 |
013 |
27 |
014 |
52 |
001 |
17 |
001 |
11 |
Small extent |
033 |
14 |
010 |
21 |
009 |
33 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
aSource: Zangari (1978).
The final criterion examined in this study called for an examination of the type of resources being used by learners. As might be expected be cause of the heavy incidence of self-directed learning, the use of books or printed material is intense (Tables 10 and 11). Note that programmed materials and TV are utilized fairly infrequently. Therefore, success as measured through the selection of non-human resources is difficult to ascertain beyond the use of fairly common forms such as books, articles, or newspapers.
Table 10. Resources Used in Learning Projects as Identified by Older Adults in Nebraska.a
Resources |
No. of Projects |
Rank |
Percent of Projects |
Books, articles, newspapers, etc. |
222 |
1 |
31.3 |
Expert |
032 |
6 |
04.5 |
Friends, Relatives, Peers |
053 |
5 |
07.5 |
Group/Group Instructor |
086 |
3 |
12.1 |
TV/Radio/Recordings/Films |
066 |
4 |
09.3 |
Displays/Exhibits/Museums |
008 |
8 |
01.1 |
Programmed Materials |
020 |
7 |
02.8 |
Mixed Sources |
221 |
2 |
31.2 |
aSource: Hiemstra (1975).
Table 11.
Resources Used in Learning Projects as Identified by Adult Educators in
Resources |
No. of Responses |
Percent of Responses |
Books, articles, newspapers, etc. |
262 |
33.8 |
Expert |
142 |
18.3 |
Friends, Relatives, Peers |
117 |
15.1 |
Group/Group Instructor |
094 |
12.1 |
Experience/Observation |
086 |
11.1 |
TV/Radio/Recordings/Films |
040 |
05.2 |
Travel |
015 |
01.9 |
Displays/Exhibits/Museums |
012 |
01.6 |
Programmed Materials |
007 |
01.6 |
aSource: Zangari (1978).
This examination of success and self-directed learning with these five criteria provide only an initial look at adults and their learning endeavors. It can be concluded from the state of the knowledge as it now exists that most adults prefer and carry out some self-directed learning each year. Only an initial understanding of the success of this learning is possible. However, it appears clear that adult and non-traditional educators must examine more closely the role they can and should be playing in facilitating such learning. The long-range livelihood of such educators and the overall effectiveness of adult learning depend on a better understanding of needed roles and products.
IMPLICATIONS
A conclusion is offered based on this study effort: Much more attention must be given by society, and more specifically by educators, to the self-directed learning phenomenon. Indeed, many educators are giving attention to the self-directed learner in terms of the development of learning resources and through the offering of alternative learning options. However, it would appear that for too many educators seem to be embracing this "new" learner as a clientele base of dollars waiting to be spent on learning. Still other educators working with adult learning programs are either pooh-poohing the self-directed learning research or viewing the at tempts to serve self-directed learners as threatening to their own head count needs. Unfortunately, there have not been many reports in the literature of efforts to think through the implications, policy needs, and programming changes related to the self-directed learner.
For example, and perhaps most important, what are the rights of the learner and the related responsibilities of the educator? Should the educator intervene in self-directed learning activities? If so, when, how often, how much, and what should be the nature of the intervention? What are the responsibilities of the educator and the rights of the learner regarding the costs of learning assistance? These and many other related questions need to have answers in the form of policies and guidelines. Perhaps, and this is not intended to be flippant, the self-directed learner needs to be protected by a code of ethics.
Another implication has to do with the creation of more and better resources for learning. High quality learning guides, programmed learning materials on a variety of subjects, inexpensive help in the form of avail able resource people throughout a community, improved correspondence courses, educational opportunities in the form of lower drop-out rates, and a greater mobilization of the entire community and its available re sources for learning are some of the possibilities. There are many other possibilities related to the development of resources.
A final implication to be discussed here deals with the differences in learning style or learner preferences of self-directed learners. Thus, the need to truly act as a facilitator of learning is as great outside the classroom setting as it is inside the classroom. Subsequently, the future training of educators who will work with or are working with life long learning programs must deal with both the facilitator and the self directed learner concepts.
For the reader's future study, several related implications were found during the literature review. They are listed in Appendix A but not in any detail. The overall sense is that good needs diagnosis methods, better resources for learning, more knowledgeable educators in terms of self-directed learning, and a more informed society that will recognize the value of self-directed learning are needed. The real challenge to educators is to truly permit the learner to grow in his or her own manner (Hesburgh & Others, 1973).
NEEDED RESEARCH
Carrying out of this research effort resulted in the development of many additional research questions. Subsequently, there arose a need to organize these questions into some meaningful way to aid future re search efforts. Borrowing some format ideas from the College Board's "Future Directions for a Learning Society" project, a framework was developed to facilitate this organizational effort. Table 12 displays the framework. Following the framework is a definition of the key concepts and a display of research questions according to the displayed format.
Table 12.
Framework for Organizing Research on Self-Directed Learning.
Research Type/Area |
Adult Students |
Adult Educators/Researchers |
Adult Education
Institutions |
Descriptive |
D-S |
D-E |
D-I |
Purposes |
P-S |
P-E |
P-I |
Content |
C-S |
C-E |
C-I |
Methods |
M-S |
M-E |
M-I |
Resources |
R-S |
R-E |
R-I |
Support |
S-S |
S-E |
S-I |
Outcomes |
O-S |
O-E |
0-1 |
Defining Framework Terms
Adult students. Research about the adult student as self-directed learner.
Adult Educators/Researchers. Research about the adult educator as a facilitator of self-directed learning or about special research that needs to be undertaken by the adult education researcher.
Adult Education Institutions. Research regarding the role of the adult or non-traditional educational institution in terms of self-directed learning.
Descriptive Research. Research that will build a basic understanding or provide knowledge benchmarks.
Research on Purposes. An examination of the aims toward which se1f directed learning is directed.
Research on Content. The substantive content that needs to be studied and/or applied.
Research on Methods. An examination of the structures and processes through which self-directed learning opportunities are provided or facilitated.
Research about Resources. Understanding the various resources avai1 able, utilized, or possible for self-directed learning.
Research on Support. Knowledge about those activities that enable the provision of learning to take place.
Research about Outcomes. Examining the results of self-directed learning
Basic Research About Adult Students
D-S-1. What are the distinguishing characteristics of non-traditional adult students and how do these differ from self-directed learner characteristics?
D-S-2. What is the nature of non-traditional learning activity and how does it compare with the nature of self-directed learning activity?
P-S-1. What motivates the self-directed learner and how do these motivations compare with similar findings about both non-traditional and traditional adult learners?
P-S-2. How can self-directed learners (and other types of learners) be helped to establish priorities, set goals, and consider options?
C-S-1. How does the content selection by self-directed learners compare with selections made by more traditional adult learners?
M-S-1. What are the relationships between cognitive style theory and self-directed learning information?
M-S-2. In what ways can cognitive style mapping be utilized for assisting self-directed and non-traditional adult learning?
M-S-3. What is the self-directed learner's relationship to the physical or social environment relative to success?
R-S-1. Can self-directed learners locate and use various resources more efficiently or effectively than other learners?
S-S-1. What support functions do self-directed learners need or provide to themselves?
O-S-1. What are the commonly perceived feedback channels for "success" attainment?
O-S-2. Is "success" the ultimate goal of the self-directed or non traditional learner or are there other important goals?
O-S-3. What is in it for the learner to be successful?
Basic Research Regarding Adult Educators/Researchers
D-E-1. What is the basic relationship between cognitive style research and information on learning projects?
D-E-2. What are both the technical and emotional aspects of non-traditional learning and are there interrelationships between the two aspects?
D-E-3. What is the relationship between stress or load and the involvement in self-directed learning?
P-E-1. What part can or should the adult educator play in helping to determine purposes of self-directed learning?
P-E-2. What part of success lies in goal setting?
C-E-1. What is the scope and different categories of non-traditional education and how do they compare to the learning projects data?
C-E-2. How does the adult education program planner deal with content in a self-directed learning setting?
M-E-1. What new methods need to be researched, developed, and evaluated for use in self-directed learning?
M-E-2. How should older methods be altered or used for self-directed learning?
R-E-1. How does the adult educator help learners match resources with needs in a self-directed learning setting?
R-E-2. What type of resource guide could be developed to aid self-directed and non-traditional learners in selection and implementation activities?
R-E-3. How could such a guide be made available and periodically updated?
S-E-1. What policy development, counseling procedures, and other support service products are required to enhance the success of the self-directed learner?
O-E-1. What are the relative bounds of the term "success"?
O-E-2. What are the criteria useful in examining the success of self directed learning?
O-E-3. How can success be measured efficiently and accurately?
Basic Research on Adult Education Institutions
D-I-1. What are institutions of higher education and the various non traditional programs doing to foster self-directed learning and learner success?
P-I-1. What are the relationships between the institutional purposes of organizations like UMA and learner goals?
P-I-2. What criteria do such institutions utilize to measure "success"?
C-I-1. How is the content of non-traditional offerings determined?
C-I-2. Is such content examined in light of self-directed learning?
M-I-1. What methods are advocated by or permissible in non-traditional institutions?
M-I-2. Do such institutions need renewal in terms of their understanding of available methods?
R-I-1. What resources are made available to learners and how are they evaluated?
R-I-2. Is there a need to create new kinds of resources?
S-I-1. How greatly does the idea of non-traditional learning differ from traditional in regard to planning, implementation, and available support for learning?
S-I-2. Are new support functions needed by self-directed learners?
O-I-1. Would a concentrated catering to self-directed learners significant1y alter the outcome expectations of non-traditional institutions?
O-I-2. Shou1d some non-traditional experiences shift to a more traditiona1 mode for better "success"?
O-I-3. What are the societal outcomes from institutions facilitating self-directed learning?
REFERENCES AND RESOURCES
1971 White House
Conference on Aging. (1973). Toward a
National Policy on Aging (Volume II, Proceedings of the Conference).
Allerton, T. D. (1974). Selected
characteristics of the learning projects pursued by parish ministers in the
Andrulis, R. S. & Bush, D. Adult cognitive styles and test performance. Educational Gerontology, 2, 173-182.
Armstrong, D. (1971). Adult learners of low-educational
attainment: The self-concepts, backgrounds, and educative behavior of average
and high learning adults of low educational attainment. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation,
Baghi, H. (1979). An investigation of the learning projects
among adults of low literacy attainment. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation,
Benson, F. B., Jr. (1974).
Learning projects of selected
administrators in
Bicknell, J. E. (1977). A proposed structure for research on adult
open learning (Theoretical Paper No. 1).
Boshier, R. (1977). Motivational orientations revisited: Life-space motives and the Education Participation Scale. Adult Education, 27, 89-115.
Cavert, C. E. (Compiler). Designing Diversity, 75 (conference
proceedings).
Cavery C. E. (Compiler). Forum 76 (conference proceedings).
Cole, J. W., Jr. &
Glass, J. S., Jr. (1977). The effect of adult student participation in program
planning on achievement, retention and attitude. Adult Education, 27, 75-88.
Coolican, P. M. (1973). The learning style of mothers of young
children. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation,
Coolican, P. (1974). Self-planned learning: Implications for the
future of adult education.
Cross, K. P. (1977). Accent on learning.
Cross, K. P. & Others.
(1975). Planning non-traditional programs.
Deep, S. D. (1977).
Student support: A total support system. In J. Kroll (Ed.), Fourth national conference on open learning
and non-traditional study (proceedings).
Denys, L. (1973). The major learning efforts of two groups of
Dickinson, G. (1971). Educational variables and participation on adult education. Adult Education, 22, 36-47.
Ebert, G. M. (1978). Correlates of success of adult open learning
college students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Eggert, J. D. (1974). An examination of goals and potential and
actual learners (Working Paper No. 1).
Eggert, J. (1974). Developing student achievement measures
(Working Paper No. 3).
Fair, J. (1973). Teachers as learners: The learning projects
of beginning elementary school teachers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Field, J. L. (1977). The learning efforts of Jamaican adults of
low literacy attainments. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Gooler,
Gross, R. (1977). Let’s
stop ignoring 80% of adult learning. In J. Kroll (Ed.), Fourth national conference on open learning and non-traditional study
(proceedings).
Gross, R. (1977). The
lifelong learner.
Guglielmino, L. M. (1977). Development of the self-directed learning readiness scale. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1977). Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, 6467A.
Haas, A. D. (1978). The exhilarating age of stressful change. TWA Ambassador, July, 23-25+.
Hesburgh, T. M., Miller,
P. A., & Wharton, C. R., Jr. (1973). Patterns for lifelong learning.
Hiemstra, R. (1975). The older adult and learning. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 117 371).
Hiemstra, R. (1976). Lifelong learning.
Johns, W. E. (1973). Selected
characteristics of the learning projects pursued by practicing pharmacists.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Johnson, E. (1973). Selected characteristics of learning projects pursued by adults who have earned a high school diploma and/or high school equivalency certificate (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1973). Dissertation Abstracts International, 34, 2332A-2333A.
Kelley, N. E. (1976). A comparative study of professionally
related learning projects of secondary school teachers. Unpublished master’s
thesis,
Kidd, J. R. (1973). How
adults learn.
Knowles, M. S. (1975). Self-directed learning.
Knowles, M. (1984). The
adult learner: A neglected species.
Knox, A. B. (1973). Lifelong self-directed education.
Knox, A. B. (1977a). Adult development and learning.
Knox, A. B. & Others. (1977b). Adult intelligence and learning ability. Adult Education, 18, 188-196.
Kroll, J. (Ed.). (1977). Fourth national conference on open learning
and non-traditional study (proceedings).
Kulich, J. (1970). An
historical overview of the adult self-learner. Paper presented at the
Northwest Institute on Independent Study: The adult as a self-learner,
Lehmann, T. (1977).
Evaluating student learning: Some findings and implications for male and female
adult learners. In J. Kulich (Ed.), Fourth
national conference on open learning and non-traditional study
(proceedings).
Long, H. B., Hiemstra, R.,
& Associates. (1980). Changing approaches to
studying adult education.
Luikart, C. (1975). Social networks and self-planned adult
learning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
McCatty, C. (1973). Patterns of learning projects among
professional men. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation,
McCatty, C. (1975).
Patterns of learning projects among physical and health education teachers. Reporting Classroom Research (
McLoughlin, D. (1971). Participation of the adult learner in program planning. Adult Education, 22, 30-35.
Meierhenry, W. C. (1978,
April). The potential relationship
between the findings of cognitive style research and findings of right and left
brain hemispheric research. Paper presented at the Adult Education Research
Conference,
Miller, N. L. (1977). Teachers and non-teaching professionals as
self-directed learners. Unpublished maters thesis,
Miller, N., & Botsman, P. B. (1975). Continuing education for extension agents. Human Ecology Forum, 6(2), 14-17.
Peters, J. M. &
Gordon, R. S. (1974). Adult learning projects: A study of adult learning in
urban and rural
Sell, G. R. (1975). An examination of learning center
effectiveness (Working Paper No. 11).
Sharon, A. T. (1971). Adult academic achievement in relation to formal education and age. Adult Education, 21, 231-237.
Smith, R. M. (1982). Learning
how to learn in adult education (Information Series No. 10).
Tough, A. (1967). Learning without a teacher: Tasks and
assistance during self-planned learning projects.
Tough, A. (1971). The
adult's learning projects.
Tough, A. (1977). Major learning efforts: Recent research and
future directions (Mimeographed Report).
Tough, A. (1978). Major learning efforts: Recent research and future directions. Adult Education, 28, 250-263.
Umoren, A. P. (1978). Learning projects: An exploratory study of learning activities of adults in a select socioeconomic group. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Nebraska, 1977). Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, 2490A.
Walsh, P. L. (1975). An exploration into reasons for
non-enrollment in SUN courses (Working Paper No. 6).
White, M. A. & Hansen, M. D. (1976). Guidelines for achievement of learner satisfaction in gerontological short term training. Educational Gerontology, 1, 193-197.
Williamson, C. (1975). Factors possibly influencing an individual’s
decision to enroll in a SUN course (Working Paper No. 7).
Zahn, J. C. (1967). Some characteristics of successful and less successful overseas community development advisors. Adult Education, 19, 15-23.
Zangari, D. J. (1978).
Learning projects of adult educators in
APPENDIX A
IMPLICATIONS SUGGESTED IN THE LITERATURE
1. Deep (1977) -- suggests reasons why self-directed learners may fail and what institutions should do to prevent such failure.
2. Gross (1977) -- suggests several national policies needed for self directed learners.
3. Lehmann (1977) -- talks about the need for special learning resources, support services, and course development.
4. Sell (1975) -- describes the value and use of learning resource centers for self-directed learning.
5. Tough (1971, 1977, 1978) -- suggests a variety of implications related to improving self-directed learning resources and opportunities.
APPENDIX B
This appendix contains a report addressed to the student enrolled in UMA courses. The UMA decision makers are urged to disseminate the report as a means of showing such learners that efforts are being made to improve the institutional capabilities.
As students enrolled or considering enrollment in UMA courses you are probably aware of the purpose of the university to make available college level courses to persons who are unable to arrange the time or resources for on-campus study. There would seem little need for lengthy discussion of today’s busy world to understand the relevance and importance of educational networks of this distant learner type. Though still in a relatively early stage of development compared to the traditional form of higher education, the permanence of this approach to education seems already assured. The sheer numbers of persons like yourselves who are busy with work and family and at the same time desire opportunity for more learning, whether for advanced degree or not, promote such an assurance.
Throughout history the doctrine of supply and demand has provided realistic insight into educational developments and their long term effect. Without becoming overly broad in considering this perspective, the UMA's bearing upon current developments in non-traditional learning should not be minimized. Though its style at present is centered upon learning through television broadcasting, its institutional structure and approach relates to other models of non-traditional learning as well. What is learned and achieved through the UMA will contribute to the entire spectrum of education in this country and conversely throughout the world. In time, the methods and distinctions between what is currently known as traditional and non-traditional education will diffuse and develop into a new state of affairs in educational terminology and practice. As today's adults and students, you are both the cause and meaning behind this steady change. It is with this reality, the importance of the UMA student, in mind that this report is written.
In recent months, researchers in adult education at
The very idea of defining success poses immediate problems because the word readily invites questions concerning in what way or in whose eyes the reference is being made. At best, success is relative to the frame work within which it is placed. The characteristics associated with it are likewise liable to similar interpretation. Everyone knows in an off hand way that success stands for the attainment of a level of achievement. As regards our educational system, success is a label given to satisfactory completion of a given course or degree orientated curriculum. Though this understanding is satisfactory on one level of social understanding, it fails to incorporate any recognizable basis on which to view individual characteristics as potential for it. On an equally important level the broad term understanding does little to account for the possible and probable variation within the reality of individual interpretation. Every one enjoys being labeled successful but not everyone is willing to rely on the label given them by others; contrary-wise, success may be personally acclaimed while not so defined by the external world. Perhaps a relevant question exists as to whether these points should make any difference given that we have at least a common understanding; at the same time another question asks if there is any way to circumvent the confusion or lack of constant interpretation between the individual, others, and the educational system of which he is a part. The significance of re search on this point necessarily addresses both the individual and the system, because above and beyond a standard definition, the relationship which they hold to one another embodies their immediate relevance, their future, and their final degree of success.
The topic of success as treated in educational writings is used most often as the end statement concerning the benefits of an organizational structure or the final point in a procedural format of learning. It is the ambition of this brief discussion to present another hopefully some what advanced view as found in self-directed learner theory. The essence of the theory relates to the motivations of the self, or adult personality, and as such relegates to the individual's problem solving interpretation and approach, the many components summed in the learning experience. Similarly, the factors and interpretive criteria of distinguishing and labeling success are also thought to rest within the individual as opposed to the educational system through which they learn. A brief survey of some major success labels is in order to better view the student and their relation to success within the S-D theory. No one doubts that the UMA holds a vested interest in its students achieving success. This measurement is limited however to statistics on course completions and ranges of received grades which maybe further analyzed with the use of student-course evaluations. In addition, focusing on the success of the student involves enormous amounts of planning administratively and especially in constructing the instructional package. The course topics, delivery systems, quality of production and credibility are all issues of concern in anticipating success. In terms of this analysis they may be seen as the learner’s institutional characteristics.
In addition to planning for the success of the student, success must be reckoned with on a purely institutional level as well. In this light it is defined by the numbers of students enrolled, satisfactory administrative functioning, adequate financial funding, and continued research and development. To take the last point a step further, the success of the UMA is characterized in relation to other educational innovations and as such its concern goes beyond its own organization and the learning of its students.
The student’s relationship to the UMA and success may be varied also. They may interpret their own success strictly in terms of the final judgment of the UMA, in conjunction with grades received for course work, or they may pass judgment independently through their own criteria of learning or personal achievement. Family and friends may exert an influence upon the student’s interpretation of success as might an enhanced work status or opportunity. As mentioned earlier the acceptance or claim of success may also seem at odds with any of the above due to the self analysis the individual decides to employ. What the view of self-directed theory expands upon is the latter part concerning the individual's will and capacity to be the judge of his or her own standing with success. This is not to suggest that the facilities, knowledge, and judgments of education al organizations such as UMA is obviated or deferred to the wisdom of the student, but rather that the student, whether acknowledged or not, maintains a judgmental understanding of his or her involvement with learning and within this sense embodies the various potentialities and components of success. Self-directed theory doesn't try to identify demographic characteristics as a predictor for the adult student. Rather it deals with the learner in terms of his maturity and the ambitions he is most likely to express from that vantage point. It follows that the educational systems which seek to serve the adult learner must necessarily observe this capacity of the adult self in order to maintain their own significance as learning centers.
Motivation on the part of the learner is far more important
than the instructor or the style of instruction they enter into. Any teacher is,
in reality, only a small part of learning; learning is achieved by, and takes
place within the learner. Both these statements strike at the heart of the
self-directed learner. The recognition the learner gives to these is similar to
the level of understanding employed with the student's personal evaluation of
success. It’s quite simply an assumption which in its true nature transcends institutional
settings. The contributions which self-directed theory makes in adopting this
view is two fold in that it structurally interprets the steps taken in everyday
life concerning learning projects and also identifies the uniqueness of the individual
aside from any possible affiliation with an educational system. In a sense, the
system's interpretation of the adult learner obviates even its own capacity to
label success in learning, although by seeking to recognize in the individual a
problem solving mode it goes beyond labeling and allows the significance of
success to rest with the learner. In this interpretation the adult learner is
at once both purposefully responsible for learning and through a range of
understanding which goes beyond the institutional framework of learning and
success, directed toward their own final interpretation. The recognition of
this on the part of the educational system which seeks to deal with adults is
as purposeful as its ultimate relation to the idea of supply and demand. Their
adoption to a truly facilitative style of educating should be, according to the
self directed learner theory, recognizable and actively supported by the adult as
student. In this style seeking success and finding success are a conscious
means of progression through a chosen subject area. The institution which seeks
to operationalize this state of learning does so with the self recognition that
its own position is not to achieve success through administrative labels but
rather as a recognition conferred upon it by its students. Though this is
suggestive of a perhaps idealistic level of understanding it is also a condition
which is dictated by the foundations of the S-D theory.
Return to first page