Page 227
OLDER ADULT LEARNING: INSTRUMENTAL AND EXPRESSIVE CATEGORIES
Roger Hiemstra
Department of Adult and Continuing Education
University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Educational Gerontology, 1: 227-236, 1976
Copyright © by Hemisphere Publishing Corporation, A Taylor and Francis Journal.
Reprinted by Permission of the
Journal’s editor
Stylistically, the article has
been converted to APA, 5th Edition.
This article examines older adult preferences for instrumental
vs expressive learning activities. Course title selections and actual learning
activity information were analyzed. Interviews with 256 Nebraskans, 55 or older, (average age was 68.11) were obtained. Hypotheses and results were (a)
predicted preference for instrumental
learning was supported; (b) greater
preference for instrumental learning by blue-collar workers and the less
educated received partial support as no differences existed for the
occupational category but individuals without college degrees preferred
instrumental courses; and (c) predicted differences in learning activity
received partial support as younger people, white-collar workers, college
graduates, nonwhites, and married people were more involved with instrumental
learning. It was concluded that more instrumental learning opportunities must
be made available to older people.
INTRODUCTION
Much has been written about the older adult and learning. Early writings focused on the premise that learning needs and capabilities decline with age. However, recent research and discussion have centered on a changing theme: declines in learning abilities and interests are less than has been historically thought. Havighurst (1972) pointed out that learning is necessary throughout life because of the continuing needs of new developmental tasks.
Thus, a variety of stereotypes and myths about the elderly are being challenged and dispelled. McClusky (1974) suggested that the elderly, in general, are active, intelligent, and involved people who have positive feelings about themselves and their potential. The purpose of the research reported here was to secure a better understanding of older adults' learning needs and potential.
The following questions served as study guides: (a) What are perceived preferences relative to instrumental and
expressive forms of education? (b) What instrumental and expressive form
of learning are older adults actually engaging in? (c) What are the
Page 228
relationships among various demographic characteristics and the perceived preferences or actual learning endeavors?
Learning needs and preferences among older people have been described in various ways. McClusky (1974) suggested five types of needs: coping, expression, contribution, influence, and transcendence. Examples of corresponding adult education programs might include (a) adult basic education for coping needs, (b) hobby courses for expressive needs, (c) leadership training for contributive needs, (d) community action education for influence needs, and (e) the study of literature or philosophy for transcendence needs.
Havighurst (1964) and Londoner (1971) described educational programs and course offerings in terms of instrumental and expressive categories of learning. Instrumental is defined as learning activities designed for effective mastery of old-age challenges and includes education on such topics as health, income, legal affairs, and adjusting to changing relationships with others. Expressive is defined as educational experiences that increase the enjoyment of life, serve to expand horizons, provide fairly immediate gratification, or facilitate opportunities for self-expression. Examples of expressive learning activities include hobby and craft instruction, travel experiences, music or art appreciation, and literature study.
Hiemstra (1972) investigated the educational needs and interests of older people. Instrumental activities were perceived by older people as more important than expressive activities. Subsequent researchers (Goodrow, 1974; Marcus, 1975; Whately, 1974) have found general support for the instrumental preference.
Dichotomizing educational opportunity into either/or categories has some drawbacks as noted by DeCrow (1974, p. 59). However, the fairly broad categories enable program planners and administrators to distinguish quite readily among learning activities according to the instrumental and expressive categories. Consequently, for purposes of this research it is hypothesized that older persons show preference for instrumental learning activities more frequently than for expressive activities. Preference is examined both by course title selection and actual learning activity.
Hiemstra (1973) also examined instrumental vs. expressive
preferences in terms of various biographical and demographic characteristics.
There were no significant differences in terms of age, sex, and urban vs. rural
categories. However, significance testing did reveal that white-collar workers
were less likely than blue-collar workers and college graduates were less
likely than non-college graduates to report instrumental course preferences.
Page 229
Thus, in this study it is hypothesized that preferences for
instrumental learning are in directions identical to those described.
Significant relationships relative to additional demographic characteristics
are reported to indicate direction for subsequent research.
Tough (1971) developed a means for determining the actual
amount of learning activity undertaken by an adult in a year. Although his
research focused on adult learners of all ages, subsequent reporting (Coolican,
1974) showed that the approach is applicable to various age groups and types of
individuals. However, there have been no reports of comparisons between the
amount of learning and preferences for instrumental or expressive forms of
learning. Thus, directional predictions in terms of demographic characteristics
are difficult to make. Utilizing Tough's methodology for discovering actual
learning activity, it is hypothesized that differences in the amount of
learning exist when instrumental and expressive categories are compared with
several demographic variables. Any differences shown will facilitate
directional predictions for subsequent research.
METHOD
Subjects
Data collection involved interviewing 256 people, 55 and
older, in a field setting. All subjects lived in Nebraska and were selected
randomly from voter registration cards in two communities and 18 rural
townships, from the rolls of two residential complexes built especially for the
elderly, and from the rolls of a Mexican-American community center. There are
built-in biases to voter registration cards. In addition, participants in
programs for special groups or residents of homes or apartments built
especially for the older person will be somewhat self-selecting. Consequently,
one study limitation is that a totally random selection of subjects probably
was not accomplished. A chi-square comparison of the study's demographic data with
U.S. Census data for Nebraska did reveal that there were no significant
differences in comparisons on sex, marital status, and occupation. However, the
study population was somewhat older, more urban, more educated and had more
nonwhites than a truly random sample of people 55 and older in Nebraska.
Of the interviewees, 41% were male, 89% were white American,
88% were middle class, 63% were married and 25% were widowed, 32% were high
school graduates or less, 44% were or had been white-collar workers, and 40%
were between the ages of 55 and 64. The average age was 68.11 years.
Page 230
Procedure
Trained interviewers asked the participants to indicate in
which of 32 courses (course titles with no interpretation were given) they
would enroll given the opportunity and if there were no preventative
constraints. The course titles were chosen from an initial pool of 75 titles
gleaned through literature searching and by securing course catalogs from five
institutions offering educational programs to the elderly. The subjects were
told nothing relative to the instrumental or expressive categories. The interviewers
also used an extensive probing technique to uncover the subjects' amount of
annual learning in terms of number of hours and number of different projects
(Tough, 1971, p. 17). (A learning project is defined as at least seven
hours of related learning within a six-month period.)
Construct validation involved the use of a panel of judges.
Each panel member (a teacher in gerontology, an administrator of gerontology
programs, and an adult education-Cooperative Extension researcher), working
independently of the other two, was given the initial list of 75 courses and
definitions of the terms instrumental and expressive. A course
title was included on the interview schedule if all three judges agreed as to
whether it were instrumental or expressive.
Concurrent validity was assessed by obtaining the
information from interviewees as to their participation in learning activities
during the year prior to the
interview. The various activities were later classified as either expressive or
instrumental in nature by the researcher and one independent judge. A
correlation of the number of course preferences in a category to the number of
corresponding actual learning projects for all individuals revealed that:
r instrumental = .254
rexpressive = .347
Both correlation coefficients are significant at the .001
level and beyond.
A telephone follow up of one person chosen randomly from
each interviewer's group of subjects was carried out approximately one month
after each interview to check the reliability of both interviewer and
instrument. There were no observed differences or discrepancies. In addition,
the total sample was split randomly into two groups. The groups were then
compared by chi-square on the
Page 231
total number of expressive and the
total number of instrumental course selections. No significant differences were
found:
|
Instrumental |
Expressive |
|
Group 1 |
629 |
402 |
|
Group 2 |
615 |
439 |
|
Totals |
1244 |
841 |
χ2
= 1.42 |
Data for the first two hypotheses were analyzed with the
chi-square test; the .05 level of confidence was utilized to determine
significant differences. Whenever a direction was predicted, the one-tailed
test of significance was utilized (Siegel, 1956, p. 13; Tuckman, 1972, p. 378).
The third hypothesis was examined by the t-test for significance in the
differences between means of two groups. The number of learning projects was
assumed to represent an interval scale so that the t-test could be used
(Kerlinger, 1967, p. 427).
RESULTS
Table 1 includes the preference information and chi-square
values pertaining to the first hypothesis. A significant preference for
instrumental learning was found both in course selections and in actual
learning projects. Approximately 60% of all course selections and learning
projects were instrumental in nature, thus supporting the hypothesis.
The second hypothesis predicted directions in the
comparisons of instrumental or expressive preferences with various demographic
characteristics. There is some support for the hypothesis (see Table 2)
TABLE
1. Instrumental and
Expressive Learning Preferences
Preference category |
Actual no. |
Expected no. |
χ2 valuea
|
Course title selection |
|
|
|
Instrumental |
1244 |
1042.5 |
77.89* |
Expressive |
0841 |
1042.5 |
|
Actual learning projects |
|
|
|
Instrumental |
421 |
346 |
32.51* |
Expressive |
271 |
346 |
|
aAssuming a null hypothesis of no difference, 50%
of the total number of course selections or learning projects could be expected
in both categories.
*p <
.001.
Page 232
TABLE
2. Comparison of Various Demographic
Characteristics with Instrumental or Expressive Learning Preferences
Characteristic |
Instrumental preferencea |
Expressive preferencea |
χ2 |
Age |
|
|
|
55-64 |
067 (054)b |
024 (020) |
.73 |
65 and older |
091 (062) |
044 (041) |
(2.57) |
Gender |
|
|
|
Female |
083 (059) |
048 (041) |
5.64** |
Male |
075 (057) |
020 (020) |
(3.71)* |
Locationc |
|
|
|
Urban |
082 (060) |
049 (041) |
7.12*** |
Rural |
076 (056) |
019 (020) |
(3.31)* |
Occupationd |
|
|
|
Blue collar |
085 (053) |
036 (027) |
.00 |
White collar |
072 (063) |
032 (033) |
(.01) |
Education |
|
|
|
College graduate |
028 (023) |
018 (017) |
1.68 |
Less than college graduate |
129 (093) |
050 (043) |
(1.18) |
Race |
|
|
|
White American |
133 (100) |
066 (053) |
6.32** |
Other |
025 (016) |
002 (008) |
(.01) |
Marital Statuse |
|
|
|
Married |
107 (088) |
035 (031) |
4.70* |
Not married |
051 (028) |
033 (030) |
(10.27)*** |
aInstrumental or
expressive preferences were determined by tabulating an individual’s total
number of selections or projects in each category. If the subject’s total for
instrumental was larger than the total for expressive, the label of
instrumental preferences was given (vice versa for expressive preferences). Not
included in the computations for this table were 30 individuals who had chosen
an equal number of instrumental and expressive courses (79 individuals had an
equal number of instrumental and expressive learning projects). Totals are not
always equal between characteristics because of some nonresponses.
bNumbers in
parentheses represent the data on learning projects. See footnote a for an explanation of how the numbers were derived.
cUrban residents
were from Lincoln, Nebraska. Rural residents were from rural areas and small
towns in Nebraska.
dHomemakers were
included in the “Blue collar” classification.
eSubjects in the “Not
married” classification were never married, widowed, divorced, or separated.
*p <
.05.
**p
< .01.
***p
< .005.
Page 233
because individuals without college
degrees were more likely to pick instrumental courses than were college
graduates. However, there was no significant difference in occupational
classification. In addition, males and rural residents were significantly more
likely to pick instrumental topics. Nonwhite and married people were significantly
more likely to make instrumental choices. Although complete support for the
hypothesis was not found, the new information should be useful for subsequent
research.
When the information on learning projects is compared by
chi-square with the demographic characteristics (see Table 2--the figures in
parentheses), the results are almost identical to the course-selection data.
Only the "race" characteristic differed, as no significant difference
was found for the learning-projects comparison.
The third hypothesis suggested that differences exist in
preferences for instrumental or expressive learning when actual learning
activity and the demographic characteristics are compared. No directional
predictions are made. Table 3 contains t-test information and shows that
the hypothesis can be supported somewhat. Only the gender characteristic and
the rural or urban status showed no significant differences in the testing for
instrumental preferences. The two age groups, the occupational classification, the
race categories, and the marital status characteristics showed no significant
differences in the t-test on expressive preferences.
The findings do supply some directional information for
subsequent research. Table 3 shows that younger subjects, white-collar workers,
college graduates, nonwhites, and married people are more likely to be engaged
in instrumental activities. Females, urban residents, and college graduates are
more likely to be engaged in expressive forms of learning.
DISCUSSION
Participation by older adults in formal adult education
courses and programs has been relatively limited according to census and other
enrollment data. Older people have not participated because of a variety of
obstacles; in addition, educational program planners and administrators often
believe that older people are not interested in formal programs and, thus, do
not encourage them to participate (Mason, 1974, pp. 71-72). The findings
presented suggest that older individuals not only have a variety of educational
preferences but also are engaging in a variety of learning projects each year.
Therefore, if education is a basic right of all people of all ages
Page 234
TABLE
3. Comparison of Various Demographic
Characteristics with the Number of Annual Instrumental or Expressive Learning
Projects
Characteristic |
Number of people |
Instrumental mean |
Instrumental St. dev. |
t-test Values |
Expressive mean |
Expressive St. dev. |
t-test Values |
Age |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
55-64 |
101 |
2.03 |
1.76 |
|
1.03 |
1.21 |
|
65 and older |
155 |
1.38 |
1.33 |
3.29** |
1.08 |
1.26 |
-.30 |
Gender |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Female |
105 |
1.58 |
1.45 |
|
1.23 |
1.30 |
|
Male |
151 |
1.74 |
1.66 |
-.85 |
0.82 |
1.10 |
2.69** |
Locationa |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Urban |
145 |
1.64 |
1.58 |
|
1.21 |
1.28 |
|
Rural |
111 |
1.65 |
1.49 |
-.04 |
0.86 |
1.15 |
2.35* |
Occupationb |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Blue collar |
142 |
1.46 |
1.38 |
|
0.96 |
1.18 |
|
White collar |
113 |
1.88 |
1.71 |
-2.17* |
1.16 |
1.29 |
-1.24 |
Education |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
College graduate |
050 |
2.14 |
1.83 |
|
1.44 |
1.22 |
|
Less than college graduate |
205 |
1.53 |
1.44 |
2.53* |
0.96 |
1.23 |
2.49* |
Race |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
White American |
227 |
1.56 |
1.56 |
|
1.03 |
1.24 |
|
Other |
029 |
2.31 |
1.23 |
-3.00** |
1.28 |
1.22 |
-1.02 |
Marital Statusc |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Married |
162 |
1.86 |
1.63 |
|
0.95 |
1.15 |
|
Not married |
094 |
1.27 |
1.29 |
3.04** |
1.24 |
1.37 |
-1.76 |
aSee Table 2,
footnote c.
bSee Table 2,
foodnote d.
cSee Table 2,
footnote e.
*p <
.05.
**p
< .01.
(White House Conference on Aging, 1973, p. 6), then more
educational opportunities must be made available to the older person.
The data also suggest that particular attention must be paid to providing educational activities of an instrumental nature. The
Page 236
methodological process of
individuals hypothetically selecting courses with a yes or a no, the lack of
sample outside Nebraska, the imprecision that undoubtedly existed in defining a
course title as either instrumental or expressive, and the fact that a course
title might connote different meanings to different people are all limitations
to the findings. The fairly large sample and the attempts at random selection
of subjects are strengths that offset some of the weaknesses. However, even if
program administrators can offer more instrumental learning opportunities, a
great effort will be required to make such opportunities widely available and
to encourage the elderly to participate in the programs and courses.
Some useful information can be obtained by examining the
differences between Tables 2 and 3. Although there were not many discrepancies
between the tables, white-collar workers were more involved in actual learning
activities, as were college graduates and nonwhite individuals, leading to
significant t-test values directionally different from those found in
the chi-square computations. Thus, as Hiemstra and Long (1974) suggest, the
work of the program planner is not made easier by discrepancies between need
expressed as preference and need demonstrated by actual learning involvement. "The program planner may have to consider more than one
indicator of need or employ more than one technique for determining need"
(p. 277).
Several issues related to the instrumental and expressive
dichotomies still must be addressed by subsequent research. Answers are needed
to questions such as how to attract participants to instrumental learning
activities, why discrepancies exist between what people state as a preference
for learning and what they actually engage in, and what differences would be
found if the instrumental and expressive categories were examined in countries
other than the United States. Additional studies will most likely provide more
information and thereby better meet the learning needs of the older person.
REFERENCES
Coolican, P. (1974).
Self-planned learning: Implications for the future of adult education. Syracuse,
NY: Educational Policy Research Center, Syracuse University Research
Corporation.
DeCrow, R. (ca
1974). New learning for older Americans. Washington,
DC: Adult Education Association of the USA.
Goodrow, B. A.
(1975). The perceived learning needs and interests of the elderly in Knox
County, Tennessee (Doctoral dissertation, University of Tennessee, 1974). Dissertation Abstracts International, 35, 5514B.
Page 237
Havighurst, R. J.
(1964). Changing status and roles during the adult life
cycle: Significance for adult education. In H. Burns
(Ed.), Sociological backgrounds of adult education. Chicago:
Center for the Study of Liberal Education of Adults, 1964.
Havighurst, R. J.
(1972). Developmental tasks and education. New
York: McKay.
Hiemstra, R.
(1972). Continuing education for the aged: A survey of needs and interests of
older people. Adult Education, 22,
100-109.
Hiemstra, R.
(1973). Educational planning for older adults: A survey of expressive vs. instrumental
preferences. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 4, 147-156.
Hiemstra, R., & Long, R. (1974. A survey of "felt" versus "real" needs of physical therapists. Adult Education, 24, 270-279.
Kerlinger, F. N. (1967). Foundations of behavioral research. New York: Holt.
Londoner, C. A. (1971). Survival needs of the aged:
Implications for program planning. International Journal of Aging and Human
Development, 2, 113-117.
Marcus, E. E.
(1975). Effects of age, sex, and socioeconomic status on
adult education participants' perception of the utility of their participation.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago,
in progress. (Personal communication with the author provided a status
report, June 27, 1975)
Mason, W. D. (1974). Aging and lifelong
learning. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 7(4), 68-76.
McClusky, H. Y. (ca
1974). Education for aging: The scope of the field and perspectives for the
future. In S. Grabowski & W. D. Mason (Eds.), Learning for aging. Washington,
DC: Adult Education Association of the USA and ERIC Clearinghouse on Adult
Education.
Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric
statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Tough, A. (1971). The adult's learning projects (Research
in Education Series No.1). Toronto: The Ontario Institute for Studies in
Education.
Tuckman, B. W. (1972). Conducting educational research. New York: Harcourt.
Whatley, L. F. (1974).
Expressive and instrumental educational interests of older
adults as perceived by adult educators, gerontologists, and older adults.
Unpublished masters thesis, University of Georgia.
White House Conference on Aging, 1971. (1973). Toward a national policy on aging (Final
Report, Vol. 2). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Created in February,
2005
Return to the gerontology information page